Psychoneurological aspects

Published on 03/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Neurology

Last modified 22/04/2025

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 1527 times

16 Psychoneurological aspects

image Clinical cases for thought

Historical development of emotional theories

Plato’s model of emotion was essentially dualist in nature. The dualism involved the existence of a soul and an earthly body it inhabited. Plato placed the emotions as a direct function of the soul, which was composed of three forces: reason, desire, and appetite. Plato envisioned the emotions as wild, uncontrollable forces in continual opposition to the controlling powers of reason. Two distinct ideas emerge from Plato’s theory of emotion. Firstly, emotions are to be contrasted with that which is rational and, secondly, that emotions play a role in psychological conflict. The second concept implies that there must also exist processes to defend against the powers of emotions.

The view that the emotions should be the slaves of reason and that reason dwelled in the divine soul made the ‘feeling’ or Platonic theory of emotion very popular among the Christian and Islamic scholars who dominated Western thinking until the late nineteenth century. René Descartes elaborated on the Platonic theory in a pamphlet entitled On the Passions of the Soul. Descartes describes the soul as the ‘switch master’ or control that allows the movement of the spirits through the body via the pineal gland in the brain. The body then experiences these movements as emotions. Emotions to Descartes were simply the experience of awareness of the spirit movements through the body. Emotions had no function, but were simply a phenomenon that occurred in response to a stimulus. For example, the movement of bodily spirits may produce the experience of fear excited by the recognition of external danger, but cannot be influenced by a cognitive appraisal of the danger or produce an appropriate behavioural response. These are two of the major criticisms of the Platonic or ‘feeling’ theory: that it can give no explanation as to how emotions can result in behaviours, and it can give no explanation as to how cognitive processes can appraise the external stimuli to alter or modify the fear response (Lyons 1992).

Regardless of the criticisms of Descartes, he was the first to suggest that some emotions might be more basic or primitive than others and listed six primary ‘passions’: wonder, joy, sadness, love, hatred, and desire. He was also the first to suggest that emotions may exist on more than one level at the same time, for example, fear and excitement (Powers & Dalgleish 1997).

William James added to the ‘feeling’ theory of emotion in his classic work Principles of Psychology in which he emphasised the physiological aspects of emotions and outlined the distinctions of each emotion which cleared the way for psychological experimentation. James was the last influential psychologist to present the feeling theory of emotion with such conviction, and with his death came the decline of the feeling theory in psychology (Powers & Dalgleish 1997).

With the decline of the dualist theory came the emergence of Darwin’s ‘survival of the fittest’ concept outlined in his Origin of Species monograph in 1859. It was Darwin’s view that emotions were a carryover from primitive man and were no longer of any use to modern humans. This theory was not widely accepted as far as it applied to the emotions, but other parts of his theory shocked most of upper society and ironically stimulated a fury of investigation that renewed interest in the biological theory of emotion.

In contrast to Darwin’s view that emotions were no longer of use but were vestigial like the appendix, and the Platonic or dualistic view that emotions were irrational, others such as Aristotle took the view that emotions have important short- and long-term functions that enable individuals to adapt to changes in their social and physical environment. Aristotle was probably the first to propose a functionalist model of emotional development. Aristotle’s theory was not widely accepted in his day, as it was overshadowed by his teacher Plato’s theories, which were much more amenable to contemporary religious leaders.

Aristotle’s most comprehensive discussion of the emotions occurs in The Art of Rhetoric. In this monograph, he outlines the relationship between an emotion and the behaviour that it produces. He also describes ten specific emotions: four positive (calm, friendship, favour, pity) and six negative (anger, fear, shame, indignation, envy, jealousy). Aristotle argued that for any emotion to arise it was necessary for three conditions to be satisfied. First, the individual must be in the appropriate ‘state of mind’ to experience the emotion; second, there must be a ‘stimulus’ to elicit the emotion; third, there must be an ‘object’ for the emotion. For example, if an individual is in a state of mind that something dangerous may happen to them, and then he/she is confronted by an assailant, they might evaluate the situation as one of impending danger. This evaluation may, in turn, result in a stimulus that produces fear. It is the evaluation of the situation and not the situation itself that stimulates the emotion. In the above example, if the assailant was not perceived as threatening to the individual, the resulting emotion may have been completely different, such as anger. Aristotle’s theory laid the foundation for the functionalist’s approach to emotions and their development.

Behavioural and social learning theories

Social learning theory emphasises the importance of modelling others’ emotional reactions as a means of developing emotional patterns and responses. The behaviourists have developed a number of various classifications of behavioural theory, depending largely on the degree to which they refer to metaphysical or epistemological claims in their explanations of emotion. The two main categories of behaviour theory are psychological behaviourism and philosophical behaviourism. The psychological behaviourist approach to emotions can best be exemplified by the theories of James Watson and B. F. Skinner. Watson, in 1919, described emotion as ‘a hereditary pattern-reaction involving profound changes in bodily mechanisms as a whole, but particularly of the visceral and glandular systems’.

In Watson’s model of emotion, only three emotions can be distinguished: fear, rage, and love. He further states that these primary emotions can only by demonstrated in the newborn. Watson’s major contribution was his finding that emotional reactions could be learned through classic conditioning (Watson & Raynor 1920).

In 1967, Etzel and Gewirtz demonstrated that operant conditioning could have an impact on emotional development. Skinner refined Etzel and Gerwirtz’s work into a complex model where emotions evolve from an operant conditioning framework, where emotion is defined by the sets of operants and reinforcers that one optimises in any given setting (Skinner 1974). One of the major criticisms of Skinner’s theory is that some emotions exhibit little or no operant behaviour. For example, grief, especially when about a loss or death, does not result in any operant behaviour because no behaviour can bring about the desired results (Lyons 1992).

One of the most influential philosophical behaviourists was Gilbert Ryle. In 1949, his work The Concept of Mind outlined that emotion can be described in four different ways: inclination, moods, agitations, and feelings. Ryle viewed inclinations as the permanent disposition state of personality. Moods, agitations, and feelings were short-term displays overlying the main inclination theme. For example, if a person has the inclination to be kind, he may still experience short-term occurrences of irritability or cruelty, which would be attributed to his mood or feeling swings.

Further expansion of the social learning theory in recent years has been accomplished by Albert Bandura, who has added a cognitive component. According to Bandura, as a person’s representational ability improves, he/she can engage in emotional self-arousal by thinking about their own emotionally charged past experiences or even by recalling the experiences of others (Bandura 1986, 1989, 1991).

One of the main criticisms of social learning/behavioural theory is that it cannot explain the emergence of emotions that have not been acquired through modelling or conditioning, but seem to appear spontaneously.

Cognitive theories

Instead of viewing emotions as central forces in the development of social interaction, cognitive psychologists view emotions as by-products of cognitive processes (Berk 1994). Several examples of cognitive theories follow, including Hebb’s discrepancy theory, Alder’s style of life theory, Epsteins cognitive-experiential self theory, and Apler’s reversal theory.

Donald Hebb (1947, 1949) explained in his discrepancy theory how distress reactions are elicited by novel stimuli. According to Hebb, when a person encounters a new stimulus, they compare it to a scheme or internal representation of a familiar object. The discrepancy between the stimulus and the internal representation determines the emotional response. Other researchers have modified Hebb’s theory, suggesting that a wide variety of emotional reactions could be explained by Hebb’s theory. For example, they argue that a positive emotion such as happiness could result from only moderate discrepancies between current stimuli and the internal scheme. Negative emotions, such as anxiety and fear, could result as the discrepancy between the present stimuli and the interval scheme widens (McCall & McGhee 1977; Kagan et al. 1978). This theory falls short when it is observed that people sometimes willingly seek out activities that are new and not in agreement with their internal scheme.

Alfred Adler, in his 1954 work Understanding Human Nature, was probably the first to integrate emotions, motivations, and cognition into one theory (Epstein 1993). According to Adler, individuals construct a belief system and a way of relating to the world which he termed a ‘style of life’. At the centre of a person’s style of life is a fictional goal which guides the individual in his attempts to overcome inferiority to gain social approval, a goal to which all humans strive. Emotions enter into Adler’s theory in two ways. Firstly, the quest for overcoming the feelings of inferiority provides an incentive for developing a style of life. Secondly, once the style of life has been developed, the emotions corresponding to that style of life are encouraged to develop. In other words, the style of life that one develops is a major determinant of the emotions that a person experiences, much like Aristotle’s ‘state of mind’ (Epstein 1993).

In the cognitive-experiential self theory (CEST), emotions are considered to be both influencing and being influenced by a person’s implicit theory of reality. The theory considers the primary emotions to be anger, sadness, joy, and affection. Cognitive affective units are constructed around the nuclei of the primary emotions. These cognitive units direct the development of critical adaptive behaviour patterns such as fighting, withdrawing, exploring, and showing affection. The development of these behavioural patterns results in emotionally rewarding experiences when they are consistent with a beneficial outcome to the individual (Epstein et al. 1992).

Apter’s reversal theory is explained in some detail here because it is one of the few theories that has a theoretical construct for encompassing changes over time in the individual which are clinically relevant for the functional neurologist. The centrepiece of reversal theory is a typology of distinct psychological states of mind. When people are in one of these states they want to experience a particular kind of emotion. The states are meta-motivational because they determine what types of experiences people want. In different states, people may react to the same stimuli in different ways, and experience distinctly different emotions. This theory focuses on how a person differs over time rather than on differences between people. Reversal theory suggests that there are eight different meta-motivational states, four pairs of opposite states. The reversal from one state to its opposite is the key feature of the theory. The two states of a pair are mutually exclusive and exhaustive; for example, a person is always in one state or the other, never both at the same time or neither state. The eight states of the reversal theory are serious–playful, compliant–defiant, power-oriented–affection-oriented, and self-oriented–other-oriented (Apter 1988). The first pair of meta-motivational states is composed of the telic and paratelic states. When in the telic state the person is primarily goal-oriented. Conversely, when a person is in the paratelic state they are best described as being playful. In this state the person does not attach much significance to what they are doing; they could not care less. The lability of a person, how readily they reverse back and forth between opposite states, varies at different times. The actual reversal process is dependent on one of three reasons: contingency, satiation, and frustration. Contingency is any change in the environment that instigates or necessitates a reversal. Satiation occurs when, in the absence of an environmental change, a reversal will eventually occur. Frustration–motivated reversals occur when a person remains in a particular state too long without achieving satisfaction (Frey 1997).

Reversal theory takes the approach of starting with motivation and experience and then interpreting the behaviour generated in light of these. Reversal theory emphasises that people are inconsistent and self-contradictory and goes so far as to say that healthy people are characterised by instability, not stability (Murgatroyd 1987).

Developing a theoretical construct for emotion

How are changes in emotional states brought about by changes or alterations in neuraxial function in humans? Can how we feel influence the function of the neuroimmune system? Emotional factors have been linked to a variety of diseases including Grave’s disease, rheumatoid arthritis, systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE), asthma, and diabetes (Koh 1998). Could these diseases be caused or precipitated by emotional factors? Several psychological states have been linked to alterations in neural activity in the limbic circuits of the brain involving the amygdala and hippocampus, which are areas historically associated with emotional response generation (Pribram & McGuinness 1975). Imbalances in emotional activation and reaction have been investigated in a number of studies (Sackeim et al. 1982; Robinson et al. 1984; Flor-Henry 1986). The advances made by these researchers have led to the discovery that cells of the immune system (lymphocytes) produce stress-associated peptides thought only to be produced in the brain and pituitary. When this finding is coupled with the discovery of neurotransmitter receptors and hormone receptors on neurons and immune cells (Blalock 1984), the existence of a bidirectional communication relationship between emotion and neuroimmune systems seems unavoidable.

How do we define and measure emotional states or changes in those states? To what extent does the interaction determine health?

What is the connection between emotion, mood, affect, and neurological function?

Campos et al. (1994) define emotion as those processes which establish, maintain, change, or determine the relation between the person and the environment on matters of significance to the person. A person’s mood may be determined by a complex interplay of the emotions that they are experiencing at any one time as per the reversal theory. ‘Mood’ therefore can be defined as the way that we feel, while ‘affect’ can be defined as how we behave as a result of our mood. Neurologically, mood and affect depend on the complex interplay among diffuse networks of the frontal lobes and subcortical circuits. They can be influenced by genetic and environmental factors, similar to any form of activity in the nervous system. Long-term changes in mood and behaviour may occur because of ‘plastic’ changes in these networks. This refers to the moulding of nerve excitability and interconnectivity referred to as neural plasticity that occurs following repeated or essential exposure to an environmental stimulus, and subsequent alteration of gene expression in the associated nerves.

Other probably genetically determined neural systems seem primed to perform a variety of innate activities called ‘fundamental functions’ in the cortex. Fundamental functions comprise diffuse, overlapping, or parallel units that contribute to more complex interactions and multimodal processing in the nervous system. The more recent definition of fundamental functions applies to frontal-subcortical and limbic circuits, which are largely intact from birth. Executive/integrative functions apply to the prefrontal circuitry, which also comprise diffuse, overlapping, or parallel influences that contribute to more complex interactions and multimodal processing in the nervous system. Fundamental functions are generally less modularised than motor or sensory functions. In other words, they are less dependent on one single area within the brain, and instead receive their input from multiple sources and generate an output that can influence the entire state of the individual. These wide-ranging functionally connected areas are often receiving similar information at the same time and are often referred to as parallel distributed processing circuits. Dysfunction associated with these networks can therefore lead to so-called ‘circuit-related disorders’ affecting complex aspects of cognition or leading to mood and behavioural changes. Therefore, abnormalities associated with fundamental or executive regions of the brain may result in either ‘negative’ or ‘positive’ symptoms. Negative symptoms or deficits generally refer to decreased expression of a normal function, whereas positive symptoms refer to release of primitive functions or new behaviours. An example of a negative symptom is depression, where a patient may exhibit withdrawal from normal activities and social interaction and less expression in their face and body movements. An example of a positive symptom can be seen clinically in paranoid schizophrenia where environmental stimulus is amplified and taken out of context of reality. Positive symptoms may also be referred to as productive symptoms, release or escape phenomena. A classic example of ‘escape’ phenomena is the exaggerated autonomic reactivity that may be observed in patients with panic or anxiety disorders. Primitive areas of the brain and brainstem are allowed to summate or increase their firing more easily because of a loss of descending reticular inhibition from the more developed frontal areas of the brain. Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) can also be viewed as an example of a release phenomena. The patient develops an irresistible urge to perform a specific action that will bring about relief of tension. While not purely seen as a ‘disinhibition’ syndrome, the patient often feels compelled to carry out an action that interferes with their daily lives. Cortical and subcortical areas implicated in mood and behavioural functions receive tonic influences from a large array of different neuronal circuits, some of which involve parallel distributed processing circuits that utilise a wide variety of neurotransmitters including dopamine, serotonin, noradrenalin, and acetylcholine. Thus the cumulative activity of these various circuits results in a temporal variation in the relative concentrations of the neurotransmitters utilised in each circuit.

The concept of parallel distributed processing is essential for healthcare practitioners to gain an understanding of the link between psychological and somatic or visceral health complaints. Not only may somatic health complaints affect the ability of an individual to exercise their normal daily routines, thus leading to altered mood and behaviour, but direct physiological connections exist that involve somatic and visceral afferents and the limbic system or ascending reticular activating systems of the brainstem and hypothalamus. One example of parallel distributed processes is outlined below, involving the rostral cingulate cortex as the limbic motor response area that responds to parallel afferent information also received by the sensory cortex. The rostral cingulate motor area (area 24) is responsible for primitive motor behaviours (fear, avoidance, etc.) mediated via the corticospinal and reticulospinal pathways. Activity in this region is also dependent on activation of the caudal cingulate motor area (area 25), which orientates the body in space. These cingulate motor areas and receptive regions of the cingulate and insular cortex that project to it are not only influenced presynaptically by subcortical and spinal neurons carrying sensory information, but they are also heavily modulated by monoaminergic neurons from the brainstem. The influence of dopamine, noradrenalin, and serotonin on sensory modulation, arousal, and orientation is complex in nature and discussed in further detail in Chapter 9.

Chronic pain and emotional responses

Functional neuroimaging using quantitative EEG (qEEG) suggests similar mechanisms between chronic pain syndromes and mood disorders in that similar areas appear to be activated in these patients. A strong tendency for overactivation of the right hemisphere or decreased left hemisphere activation has been identified in patients who demonstrate negative behaviour or affect (Davidson 1992). Similar findings are often present in patients with chronic pain syndromes suggesting that pain, stress, and negative emotions may share common influences on association areas of the cortex concerned with contextual processing. Pain-related circuits in the brain, particularly those associated with ‘older’ pathways, tend to adapt and undergo plastic changes when closely associated with a behaviour or an emotion. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain has been used to identify the neural networks involved in aversive conditioning, and anticipation of visceral pain. Actual and anticipated visceral pain elicited similar cortical responses. This demonstrates similarities with the principle that imagined movements through visual imagery can strengthen learning of new motor sequences. At some point in the sequencing of neural activity, motor execution is inhibited or restrained during motor imagery. However, actual, imagined, and perhaps anticipated movements or perceptions may share the same neural networks up until this point of restraint. With respect to mood and affective disorders the anticipation of negative consequences associated with illness, injury, or social and environmental stimuli may therefore induce a new ‘virtual reality’ that bears all the hallmarks of ‘actual’ disease and disability.

The amygdala receives direct synaptic connections from thalamic and spinal cord neurons that are involved in aversive conditioning and fear-potentiated behaviour. Approximately half of the neurons projecting to limbic or striatal regions terminate in the hypothalamus. This suggests that somatic and visceral afferents to the spinal cord are intrinsically linked to higher limbic centres and may profoundly affect behaviour. Since mood and behaviour are inextricably dependent on the central integrated state of both limbic and higher cortical centres, presynaptic influences on these areas may have a central role in the aetiology of mood and behavioural disorders. Increased activation of pain pathways has already been mentioned, particularly with respect to activation of the ‘older’ pain pathways. However, other sensory systems share similar relationships with the limbic system, including the visual, auditory, vestibular, olfactory, and gustatory systems. An overview of the relationship between the auditory system and mood and behaviour is detailed below.

Brain imaging studies have shown similar activation patterns in chronic tinnitus sufferers and chronic pain sufferers who also suffer from depression. Plastic changes in receptors associated with brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neurokinins have also been identified in the hippocampus of these patients.

Mood and behavioural problems may be associated with altered ascending inputs from various subcortical circuits such as those in the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN) and somatic or vestibular processing pathways. It has been postulated that the cause of tinnitus may represent a distributed phenomenon with the possibility of dysfunction in a variety of pathways individually or simultaneously rather than damage at one location. Thus, interactions between many brain regions may be the cause. The same principle is likely to apply to mood and behavioural problems. The auditory portions of the DCN and vestibular nucleus share some similarities with respect to their apparent influence on limbic/reticular nuclei and therefore mood. The output from both of these nuclei may be heavily influenced via interactions with cervical spine afferents. In a detailed review of the literature concerning the effect of rehabilitation exercises on vestibular adaptation, Black and Pesznecker (2003) found that vestibular rehabilitation outcome is negatively affected by anxiety, depression, and cognitive dysfunction, suggesting a role for mood, affect, and cognition in modulating balance and/or spatial processing. A number of mechanisms linking balance control and anxiety have been found:

The most likely mechanism for these interactions is that the serotonergic pathways mediated by the raphe nuclei and receptors of targets of the parabrachial nucleus calibrate the sensitivity of affective responses to aversive aspects of motion.

When alterations in emotion are related to changes in the neuroimmune system, many variables arise. Researchers have attacked the problem by breaking the broad concept of emotion into smaller, more manageable sections, mainly positive and negative emotional states. Knapp et al. (1992) asked subjects to recall and relive maximally disturbing situations in their lives which they classified as negative emotions and maximally pleasurable situations in their lives which they classified as positive emotions. They found that negative emotional states promoted significant declines in mitogenic lymphocyte reactivity followed by a return to pre-emotional levels. They also found a similar decrease in cytotoxic T-cell function with negative emotional stimuli. Modulation of the immune system in these studies was thought to have occurred via nervous system function. Psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia have repeatedly been shown to have altered immune system function, and some investigators have suggested that immunological dysfunction may, in fact, contribute towards the multifactorial aetiology of schizophrenia via bidirectional parallel circuit feedback systems (Kirch 1993; Syvalahti 1994; Rothermundt et al. 1998). Several clinical conditions are outlined below with some discussion of their neuroimmunological relationships.

Mania

Affect, which is another way to describe emotion, gives richness and meaning to our experience of the world around us and many would say is an indispensable dimension of our humanism. When the behaviours or feelings produced by emotions become inappropriate or extreme they can be the source of overwhelming psychological distress (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). Disturbances in mood which result in intense feelings of sadness or elation that are unrealistic and last over a prolonged period of time result in depression or mania, respectively.

The affective disorders or disorders of feeling have been recognised and written about since the history of medicine has been recorded. Melancholia, which is another term for depression, was noted by Hippocrates in the fourth century BC and has been found referenced as early as the first century AD. Some very famous people have fallen victim to depression including Abraham Lincoln and Winston Churchill. Even though these conditions have been investigated for centuries they still remain something of a mystery. Some disturbances in affect can be caused by inappropriate responses to intense or chronic mental stress.

How do people usually respond to mental stress? Some of us try to distract our attention from the effects of the stress by becoming feverishly active and energetic; others accomplish the distraction by surrounding themselves with people or by constantly going out to parties or social events. Doing unusual amounts of work is another tactic. In short, many of us respond to stress in a way that resembles manic behaviour. When does this behaviour become pathological in nature?

Overactivity becomes manic when it becomes extreme, prolonged, and uncontrollable. Manic individuals are hyperactive, talkative, and endlessly energetic and usually perform these behaviours in bursts of activity that eventually result in a burnout period of exhaustion. They find great superficial pleasure in people and things that never interested them before until their attention is turned to another more interesting topic. In the process of these bursts of mania, their self-image becomes grossly inflated. They tend to ignore their limits, believing they can do anything. They love and admire themselves without reservation and ironically they are often irritable, unhappy, and reckless in their actions (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

Depression

Most people report that they have gone through periods of depression or extreme dejection at some point in their lives. They admit to ‘feeling sorry for themselves’ and may report the following symptoms:

These symptoms that most people have reported experiencing are similar to the symptoms of pathological depression. It is essential to grasp the concept that most psychological dysfunctions, including pathological depression, are determined based on the degree of symptom expression. Pathologically depressed patients often show a degree of utter despair and hopelessness that is foreign to the experience of most people (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

People with depression live in a state of sheer hopelessness, in which there exists no source of pleasure, and in some, no reason for living. Some people who are depressed do, in fact, kill themselves. They may experience delusions and hallucinations which do not occur in ‘normal’ periods of depression. For example, they fear the imminent destruction of the world, or that terrible tragedies are in store for them or their loved ones.

Persons who undergo one or more major depressive episodes with no intervening manic episodes are classified as major depressive in nature. In the United States the prevalence of major depression is about 3% for men and about 9% for women and the lifetime risk, which is defined as the chance of experiencing at least one episode of major depression, is 12% for men and 26% for women (Boyd & Weissman 1981). Depression is second only to schizophrenia as the primary condition for admissions to mental hospitals (Woodruff et al. 1975).

Some groups of people, such as low socioeconomic classes, of both sexes, middle-aged and elderly, and women in general are more susceptible than others to developing depression. It is distressing to note that young people have recently started to increase their prevalence of depressive episodes. Even the incidence in infants has been reported to be on the increase.

In about 50% of the cases the first episode is also the last episode. They have no recurrence. However, for the remaining 50% the depression will come and go many times (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). The episodes may occur in clusters or be separated by many years of normal function. In most cases adjustment back to a normal life occurs relatively quickly. However, in about 20% of people, return to their normal premorbid state following a major depressive episode does not occur. Why these people do not return to their normal or premorbid state becomes understandable to a certain extent when consideration is given to the effects that a depressive episode can have on their lives. A major episode of depression often erodes self-confidence, disrupts family and marital relationships, interferes with progress at school or work, and alters other people’s expectations of the depressed individual. Thus, the event itself sets up a vicious circle of reoccurrence by the state in which it leaves the individual.

Classification

Several classifications of depression have been developed. The following classification is based on the age or stage of development of the individual.

Depression and neuroimmune function

Depression has been shown to be related to schizophrenia (Crow 1984), herpes simplex virus (Halonen et al. 1974), Ebstein–Barr virus (Amsterdam et al. 1986), human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) type I (Levy & Bredesen 1989), several autoimmune diseases (Johnstone & Whaley 1975), leukaemia (Greene 1954), and a variety of cancers (Persky et al. 1987). These relationships suggest disorders of the neuroimmune system in some fashion or other.

The role of neuroimmune function in depression has attracted attention for many years (Calabrese et al. 1987). Depressive illness poses a major public health problem with 2% to 3% of the population hospitalised or seriously afflicted at any one time; in this light, many investigators have approached depression from a neuroimmunological prospective (Stein et al. 1991). Many studies document that patients with depression show reduced immune function throughout a wide variety of immune function measures. Stein et al. (1985) found that in-patients with depression have poorer blastogenic responses than non-depressed controls. Depressed patients have also been shown to have a lower percentage of helper T-lymphocytes than non-depressed controls (Krueger et al. 1984). Irwin et al. (1990) showed that when compared to normal controls, men with major depressive disorder were associated with a 50% reduction in T-cell cytotoxicity. Not all research supports the above findings. Stein et al. (1991) reported in a comprehensive review that out of eight studies that they looked at only one found lymphopenia to be significant in depressive patients studied. Out of five additional studies examined by the same authors, again only one showed an alteration in neutrophil counts in depressed patients. Immune function studies in patients with depression have also been explored. Irwin et al. (1987b) found a decrease in cytotoxicity of natural killer cells in depressed patients as compared to controls. Other studies relating to natural killer cell cytotoxicity did not find any significant difference between controls and depressed patients (Mohl et al. 1987; Schleifer et al. 1989). It is important to consider that depression is a diagnosis derived from diagnostic criteria composed of many variables (DSM IV). It is also necessary to realise that a diagnosis of clinical depression does not preclude the coexistence of other dysphoric mood states including anxiety and/or hostility. Depressed patients may have different combinations of symptoms and still be diagnosed with depression. For example, a patient may have predominately psychological symptoms such as self-reproach, difficulty concentrating, loss of interest, and recurrent thoughts of death or suicide. In contrast, another patient may have predominately vegetative symptoms including poor appetite and weight loss, sleep disturbance, loss of energy, or psychomotor agitation. These two types of patients may have vastly different neuroimmunological alterations as a result of the same diagnosis. This heterogeneous population may in some way explain the inconsistent results obtained when populations are not controlled for variables such as those described.

The degree of immunosuppression may also be related to the severity of the depression studied (Stein et al. 1985). Kemeny (1994) found that higher levels of a depressed mood were associated with lower numbers of cytotoxic T cells. In both types of studies, functional and enumerative, no consistent results have been reported for any cell types or subtypes studied. This inconsistency, as described above, may be a result of both conceptual and methodological concerns that limit the interpretation and generalisation of these study results. Few of the studies distinguished between any of the many recognised types of depression. As previously stated, the severity of depression may play a role in the cytological response and distinguishing between types may be necessary for more consistency in results. Small sample size and no controls for age, ethnic backgrounds, gender, and medication status may also have led to the inconsistent results reported in the above-mentioned studies.

Loneliness is a paradigm closely related to depression and several studies have found similar results of decreased immune response and function in lonely subject populations studied. A team led by Kiecolt-Glaser in 1984 found in separate studies that both medical students (Kiecolt-Glasser et al. 1984a) and psychiatric patients (Kiecolt-Glasser et al. 1984b) suffering from loneliness had lower cytotoxic killer cell activity and lymphocyte responses to mitogen stimulation. It is clear that more research into this area is necessary to determine the true effects and modulating variables of depression and loneliness on neuroimmune system function, especially studies controlling for degree of severity and types of symptoms expressed in study populations.

Is depression related to cortical asymmetries in function?

Several studies have demonstrated increased prevalence of depressed mood in patients who have suffered damage to the left frontal region of the brain compared to patients with lesions on the right. In addition, it was noted that the more anterior the location of the lesion, the more likely the patient would experience depression (Davidson 1992). The left brain is thought to be responsible for brain functions assigned to the will to perform or act. Right-handed reaching and positive affect are taken to be the collective manifestation of an approach system centred in the left frontal region. Damage to the left frontal region results in behaviour and experience which might best be characterised as a deficit in approach. Therefore, patients with damage or dysfunction involving the left hemisphere may lose pleasure and interest in people or objects and have difficulty initiating voluntary action. During withdrawal-related emotional states such as fear and disgust, the right anterior regions of the brain are activated. PET brain imaging has identified hyper-responsive regions of the right hemisphere that project to the amygdala in panic-prone patients. In order for depression to manifest clinically, more than just an injury to the left cortex is necessary. The person must also be exposed to the right set of environmental stimuli for the depression to be initiated. In a patient with left anterior cortical damage, depressive symptomatology would be expected only if that patient were exposed to the requisite environmental stresses. Left anterior damage is not in itself sufficient for the production of depressive symptomatology. We would therefore not expect all patients with left frontal damage to show depressive symptomatology. Only those exposed to an appropriate set of environmental stresses would be expected to show the hypothesised final state (Davidson 1992). Quantitative EEG studies have also proven to be effective in measuring changes in cortical activity associated with depression. Increases in left frontal alpha power are consistent with decreased activation of this area of the brain, along with frontal hypocoherence or decreased synchrony between the hemispheres.

Suicide

In any discussion on depressive disorders, some discussion on suicide must be included because of the strong association between suicide and affective disorders. In his work The Savage God, Alvarez states, ‘The processes which lead a man to take his own life are at least as complex and difficult as those by which he continues to live.’ Yet we know that a major factor in deciding whether to take one’s own life is the feeling of hopelessness that occurs in depression. In a study of successful suicides, experimenters found that 94% of them had gone through episodes of serious depression (Robins et al. 1959).

The statistics on suicide are difficult to obtain because some who commit suicide want it to look accidental for a variety of reasons, some of which include insurance claims, or to spare families the shame that suicide usually brings. Some researchers have estimated that as many as 15% of all traffic deaths in the USA were suicides (Finch et al. 1970). Suicide is one of the top ten causes of death in the United States. About 1% of the population has attempted suicide at least once (Epstein 1974).

Among young people in New Zealand, between 15 and 24 years of age, it is considered the second leading cause of death. Three times as many women attempt suicide as men, but three times as many men than women actually succeed in killing themselves. Two times as many single people kill themselves as married people do. The most common profile to commit suicide is a native-born male, in his forties or older, who is depressed or ill and kills himself by hanging, shooting, or poisoning by CO (Shneidman & Farberow 1970).

Is there a certain personality type more likely to kill themselves than another? Apparently not! Freud even had suicidal thoughts throughout his life (Jones 1963). There are, however, certain types of reasoning that can lead people to kill themselves (Shneidman & Mandelkorn 1970).

Common stereotypical beliefs about suicide

1. People who threaten to kill themselves will not carry it out; only the silent types will actually do it. This is quite untrue. About 70% of those who threaten suicide actually attempt it (Stengel 1964). In other words, when someone threatens to kill themselves it must be taken seriously.

2. People who attempt suicide and fail were not serious about it in the first place. This statement is also untrue. The statistics tell us 75% of all successful suicides have made a previous attempt (Cohen et al. 1966). As many as 12% of those who experienced a failed attempt will make a successful second attempt within 3 months (Shochet 1970).

3. People suffering from depression should not be questioned about suicidal thoughts. Many people have held the view that depressed people should not be questioned about suicidal thought in the fear that this questioning will put the idea into their head, or it will reinforce it if it is already there. It is now believed that questioning these people directly can help them overcome their feelings and at the very least offer information to direct a therapeutic direction.

4. When a person commits suicide, family and friends are often astonished. This statement is often true. Friends and family will often make statements such as ‘He was in such good spirits’; ‘She had so much to live for.’ This highlights the fact that friends and relatives are often oblivious to the clues that most people contemplating suicide give out before they kill themselves.

Psychotic versus neurotic

Psychological disorders can be distinguished based on severity as either neurotic or psychotic. This distinction has traditionally hinged on the matter of reality contact, which is the ability to perceive and interact in one’s environment in a reasonable manner. Neurotics may be severely incapacitated but they can seldom be characterised as out of contact with reality. Psychotics, on the other hand, demonstrate a perception of reality that is grossly distorted. Many psychotics have hallucinations and delusions; others withdraw into themselves, creating their own private world (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). In psychosis, their sense of reality is so severely impaired that they cannot achieve even the most marginal adaptive functioning. For this reason most psychotics are hospitalised.

Two further classifications of depression have been established based on whether the person expresses neurotic or psychotic tendencies. In neurotic depression, the person may experience extreme anguish but still know what is going on around them. In psychotic depression, the person may experience hallucinations, delusions, or extreme withdrawal, which effectively cuts the tie between the individual and the environment, precluding adaptive functioning. There are some psychologists who have expressed the opinion that neurosis and psychosis are not two different entities but rather two ends of the spectrum of the same disorder (Beck 1967).

Psychosis

The psychoses are usually divided into two broad categories:

The functional psychoses are divided into three classes:

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is marked by a variety of symptoms and actions including bizarre behaviour, social withdrawal, and severe distortion of thought, perception, and affect. Schizophrenia is probably not a single entity involving a single part of the brain. On the contrary, it is most likely a group of disorders which differ widely in aetiology and symptomatology. About 1 in every 100 people has had or will have a schizophrenic episode. There are about 1 000 000 active schizophrenics in the USA (Berger 1978). Half of all beds in mental hospitals are taken by schizophrenics. Half of all schizophrenics released from mental institutions will return within 2 years of release (Gunderson et al. 1974).

The fundamental symptoms of schizophrenia can be remembered by the pneumonic of the four ‘A’s:

The secondary symptoms of schizophrenia may include:

The development of schizophrenia may present in phases which include:

There are five types of schizophrenia commonly recognised clinically:

Neurosis

Originally, this was thought to be an organic disorder involving a general affliction of the nervous system that produced various forms of bizarre nervous behaviour. Throughout the nineteenth century, those people who were demonstrably sane but nevertheless engaged in rigid and self-defeating behaviours were labelled neurotic and thought to be victims of some identified neurological dysfunction. Around the turn of the century, this biogenic view of neurosis was gradually replaced by Freud’s psychogenic view (Freud 1894). To Freud, neurosis was not due to organic causes, but rather to anxiety. As repressed memories and desires threatened to break through from the unconscious to the conscious mind, anxiety occurred as a danger signal to the ego. The neurotic behaviour that developed was either the expression of that anxiety or defence against it. This view has held until the past few decades where there has been a growing opposition to using the term neurosis to describe all of the anxiety disorders due to the fact that not all of the neuroses express anxiety directly.

Anxiety

Anxiety is a multistate phenomenon which involves subjective state changes, a state of physiological arousal, and a state of cognitive disruption. For this reason, anxiety is difficult to measure accurately. Tests of these different dimensions of anxiety often disagree with one another because we have no reliable yardstick with which to measure anxiety. Without such a yardstick it is hard to use anxiety in making diagnostic distinctions. Anxiety is not limited to so-called neurotics; normal people feel it too, as do psychotics, depressives, and sexual deviants. Anxiety can be experienced in three basic patterns:

Anxiety involves three basic components:

Anxiety disorders involve a state of fear and apprehension that affects many different areas of functioning. The anxiety disorders include the following:

Generalised anxiety disorders (GAD)

The main feature of this disorder is a chronic state of diffuse unfocused anxiety. People with this disorder cannot say what the cause of their anxiety is. All they know is that they feel a persistent sense of tension and dread (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). People with GAD are continually on edge, waiting for something dreadful to happen, either to themselves or to those they care about. Eventually they may develop secondary anxiety, which involves a state of anxiety about their anxiety, fearing that their condition will cause them to develop health problems, lose their jobs, default on their mortgages, go crazy, and so forth.

The subjective feelings produced in these individuals spills over into their cognitive and physiological activities in such a way as to disrupt their normal existence. The person finds it hard to concentrate, make decisions, and remember commitments (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). At the same time, chronic muscle tension and heightened arousal in the nervous system give rise to numerous physiological complaints such as:

The diagnosis of generalised anxiety disorders includes:

Three or more of the following six symptoms must also be present:

The affects of anxiety on neuroimmune function

High levels of anxiety have been shown to occur concomitantly with blunted T-cell blastogenesis (Fawzy 1995) and inhibited lymphocyte response (Linn et al. 1981). Koh and Lee (1998) found that untreated patients with anxiety disorders showed significantly reduced lymphocyte proliferative response and decreased interleukin-2 response when compared to normal controls. However, in the same study they could find no significant difference in T-cell cytotoxicity between groups. Another study by Surman et al. (1986) found no significant difference in lymphocyte proliferative response between panic patients and control groups.

Although most studies have shown a decrease in immune function with high anxiety levels, some studies have found the opposite, that high levels of anxiety can be associated with increased immune function (Koh 1997). Koh found that students undergoing anxiety from highly competitive exams actually had enhanced lymphocyte proliferation. These finding may reflect the fact that anxiety is a very complex emotional state to measure. Izard (1972), using the differential emotions scale (DES), determined that anxiety is experienced as a variety of emotions including fear, guilt, sadness, and shame. The inconsistent findings relating the immune response to anxiety may be due to the fact that different anxiety characteristics are being measured, which results in different responses of the immune system.

Another explanation for the inconsistent findings described above, as outlined by Koh (1993), is that the different researchers were measuring different severities of anxiety, mainly acute anxiety and subacute anxiety. Koh suggests that subclinical anxiety may be associated with increased immune function, which may be a transient phenomenon occurring prior to any down-regulation of immune function shown to occur with clinical anxiety.

Panic disorder

In panic attacks, the feeling of anxiety mounts to an almost unbearable level. The person sweats, feels dizzy, trembles, shivers, and gasps for breath. Their pulse quickens and their heart pounds. Above all, there is a feeling of inescapable doom; the person may feel that he or she is about to die, go insane, or commit some horrible act (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). These attacks usually last several minutes, though they may continue for hours. When the attack subsides, the person often feels exhausted, as if he or she has been involved in a traumatic experience. In cases where the panic attack is triggered by a phobia, it is referred to as a phobic attack. However, in instances where these attacks occur in the absence of any phobic stimulus, it is referred to as panic disorder.

Since the panic attacks are unpredictable, patients cannot go anywhere. The movies, the grocery store, a restaurant are all out of bounds because these people fear that they may have another attack in front of everyone. Consequently, victims of panic disorder may cease to go anywhere and develop the disorder known as agoraphobia. Agoraphobia is anxiety of being in places or situations from which escape might be difficult, or where help may not be available in the event of having an unexpected panic attack. Fears commonly involve clusters of situations, like being alone, being in a crowd, standing in a line, travelling on a bus, or sitting in a classroom (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

The mnemonic for panic disorder is ‘Students Fear the 3 ‘C’s’:

Diagnosis of panic disorder

The American Psychiatric Association’s (APA) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV) classifies panic disorder as an abrupt onset of fear or discomfort that peaks in approximately 10 minutes and includes at least four of the following:

Diagnosis also requires that the panic attacks recur every 2 weeks or that a single attack is accompanied by at least 1 month of persistent concern about future attacks, worry that the attacks will cause physical illness or insanity, or significant changes in behaviour related to the attacks.

There is no consensus about the initial appearance of panic. The commonplace description of panic patients is that attacks ‘come out of the blue’. There are two professional views of how panic develops. The first, a biological view, is that panic initially appears almost fully fledged in its physical manifestations. It is recognised, however, that following the initial panic manifestations, cognitively driven elaborations and amplifications may subsequently develop. The second view is that physical perturbations, in combination with certain attitudes/cognitions which may or may not be fully conscious, lead to the experience of panic.

The ‘cognitions’ in people experiencing panic attacks may be considered as triggers only, as the underlying mechanism of panic attacks may be related to a heightened sensitivity of limbic circuits that are ‘wound up’ by a loss of inhibition. This may occur due to metabolic or neurotransmitter imbalances, altered afferentiation from the periphery, or decreased effect of more central influences due to hemisphericity, diffuse axonal injury following concussion or whiplash, and environmental exposures such as chemical and emotional stressors.

Obsessive–compulsive disorder

An obsession is a thought or an image that keeps recurring or returning to the mind. The individual may consider the thought or image a senseless activity and may even find the recurring nature or the thought itself extremely unpleasant in nature. A compulsion is an action that the individual feels compelled to repeat again and again, to reduce the level of anxiety that seems to continuously build up within them. Usually the person has no conscious desire to perform the act but does so anyway (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

Mild obsessions strike many of us from time to time. We may dwell repeatedly on some song lyric or a thought may keep running through our mind. But these minor obsessions pass and do not prevent us from getting on with our normal lives. Pathological obsessions do not pass. They keep recurring and recurring, day in and day out. Usually, pathological obsessions take the form of a violent or demoralising quality, such as a mother obsessing with the idea of drowning her baby in the bath or a man obsessed with the fear he will masturbate in public (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

Compulsions tend to fall into two categories (Rachman & Hodgson 1980):

It is important to make the distinction between true obsessive–compulsive disorders and conditions such as compulsive gamblers or compulsive eaters. True obsession and compulsion does not bring pleasure to the victims. Compulsive gamblers or compulsive eaters may be deeply pained by the consequences of these excesses. Nevertheless, they take pleasure in eating and gambling, so these are not true obsessive–compulsive behaviours (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

Post-traumatic stress disorder

These disorders are acute psychological reactions to the person’s exposure to an intensely traumatic event. For example, the person may be involved in or witness an assault, natural disaster, an airplane crash, a devastating fire, torture, and/or bombings. This condition differs from other anxiety disorders in that the source of the stress is an external event of an overwhelmingly painful nature, so to a certain extent it may seem justified or normal to feel this way. These disorders can be extremely debilitating. For example, victims may go on for days, weeks, or months re-experiencing the traumatic event in their minds. They may show a diminished responsiveness to their present surroundings, a sort of ‘emotional anaesthesia’. They may find it difficult to respond to affection. They may develop insomnia, decreased sex drive, and heightened sensitivity to noise. They also have greater expression of depression.

The traumatic event is persistently re-experienced through one or more of the following:

Diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder requires that there be persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with trauma, as well as persistent symptoms of the following:

Symptoms must be present for greater than 1 month for the diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder to apply.

Somatoform disorders

The primary feature of somatoform disorders is that psychological conflicts take on a somatic or physical form. Some patients complain of physical discomfort, stomach pains, breathing problems, and so forth. Other patients show an actual loss or impairment of some normal physiological function; for instance they are suddenly unable to walk or swallow (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). In either case, there is no organic evidence to explain the symptom, while there is evidence that the symptom is linked to a psychological cause.

There are several types of somatoform disorders. We will look at three forms:

Hypochondriasis

The primary feature of this condition is a continuous, inescapable fear of disease. The fear is maintained by constant misinterpretation of physical signs and sensations as abnormal and representing clinical signs or symptoms of a disease process. Hypochondriacs have no real physical disability or disease process that can be clinically proven to exist.

Often when hypochondriacs present at a health professional’s office, they have already diagnosed their condition, for they are usually avid readers of medical textbooks, and have spent countless hours on the Internet searching their symptoms for the diseases that cause similar symptoms. When the healthcare professional tells them that they can find no physical cause for their symptoms and there is actually nothing wrong with them they are disappointed and will often change healthcare professionals until they find someone who at least agrees to perform more tests to prove that they do, in fact have, a disease or condition. Often these people go through several healthcare professionals per year.

It should be noted that hypochondriacs do not fake their symptoms. They truly feel the pains they report. They cannot be reassured by the medical evidence presented to them that their fears are irrational (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). However, these fears do not have the bizarre quality of the disease delusions experienced by psychotics, who will report that their feet are about to fall off or their brains are shrivelling. Instead, hypochondriacs tend to confine their anxieties to more ordinary syndromes, such as heart disease or cancer. Eventually, they generally focus on a single disorder.

Somatisation disorder

This disorder is also known as Briquet’s syndrome (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). This condition is characterised by numerous and recurrent physical complaints which have persisted for several years and have caused the person to seek medical help, but for which no medical or scientific explanation can be given. This condition resembles hypochondriasis in that it involves symptoms with no demonstrable physical cause. Yet the two disorders differ in the focus of the patient’s distress. What motivates the hypochondriac is the fear of disease, usually a specific disease. The symptoms are troubling only because they indicate the presence of that disease. In contrast, it is actually the symptoms themselves that concern the patient with somatisation disorder.

The diagnosis of this condition can be extremely complicated and frustrating for a number of reasons. Firstly, the patient usually describes dramatic and exaggerated symptoms. Secondly, in the case of hypochondriac’s, they often fear one particular disease, and therefore their complaints tend to be fairly limited. In someone with somatisation disorder, on the other hand, the complaints are many and varied. In fact, this condition requires 12 to 14 different kinds of complaints for this diagnosis to be given. The occurrence is more common in females than males and 1% of women may develop some form of this disorder throughout their lives (Bootzin & Acocella 1984).

Conversion disorder

In hypochondriasis and somatisation disorder, there is no real physical disability, only a fear of, or complaints about, an illness or disability. In conversion disorder, there is an actual disability. The disability usually includes the loss or impairment of some motor or sensory function (Bootzin & Acocella 1984). Formerly known as hysteria, conversion disorder has played a central role in the history of psychology (see Quick Facts 16.4).

Like the symptoms in hypochondriasis and somatisation disorder, conversion symptoms are not supported by medical evidence. But neither are they faked. The symptoms are involuntary responses, which are not under the person’s conscious control. At the same time, they contradict the physiological facts; for instance, upon examination, the eyes will be found to be perfectly free from defect or damage and yet the person will not be able to see. Thus, conversion disorders differ from psychosomatic disorders such as ulcers in that in psychosomatic disorders there is an observable medical dysfunction.

Conversion symptoms vary considerably. Among the most common are:

Many patients with conversion disorder seem completely unperturbed by their symptoms. Whereas most people would react with horror to the discovery that they were suddenly blind or could no longer walk, the conversion patient is relatively unconcerned. This phenomenon is referred to as ‘La belle indifference’. In fact, they are typically eager to discuss their symptoms and will describe them in the most full and vivid terms to anyone who will listen. They also do not seem particularly eager to part with their symptoms.

In conversion disorder patients the patient’s body appears to be in good health. Biologically and physiologically, conversion patients can do whatever it is they say they cannot do. However, either by trickery or under hypnosis or the influence of drugs they can perform the task that they deny the ability to perform. Further evidence for their lack of organic pathology is that the symptoms are often selective. For example, conversion epileptics seldom injure themselves or lose bladder control during a seizure as do true epileptics. Likewise, in conversion blindness, patients rarely bump into things. It must be reinforced at this time that conversion patients by definition are not consciously refusing to use parts of their body. Their response is involuntary.

Diagnosis of this disorder is difficult. First, malingering, the conscious faking of a symptom in order to avoid some responsibility, must be ruled out. The second and much more difficult task is ruling out an actual organic disorder. Certain signs may suggest conversion disorder. These include:

Stress

In 1936, Selye reported that laboratory animals presented a common reaction to exposure to noxious stimuli such as cold, heat, X-rays, adrenaline, insulin, or muscular exercise. Selye called this specific biological response stress and anything that induced this response a stressor. Since Selye first introduced his stress/stressor theory, this concept has undergone a progressive evolution from animal models consisting of only physical stressors to human models involving the distinction between physical stress and psychological stress. Studies carried out by Lazarus (1966) were instrumental in the evolution of the above concept by describing the fundamental role of the central nervous system, and of psychological factors in the response to stressors. The first scientist to investigate the role of the immune system to stress conditions appears to be Ishigami in 1919. While studying the effects of chronic tuberculosis, he observed a decrease in the phagocytic activity of leukocytes during periods of the greatest psychological stress. Many studies have since shown the interconnection of the central nervous system and the immune system in response to stress (Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser 1991; Plotnikoff et al. 1991; Seymour 1993; Madden & Felten 1995; Bondi & Zannino 1997). Most authors today consider the neuromodulation of host immunocompetence the principal system involved in the mediation of pathogenic effects of psychosocial factors (Bondi & Zannino 1997), although modulation of anatomical microfunctional barriers and host modulation of the infectious agent may also play a less important role (Cohen et al. 1991; Evans & Edgerton 1991). Human studies involving psychological stress have mostly focused on ‘physical’ stressors such as sleep deprivation and noise (Palmbald et al. 1976, 1979; Weisse et al. 1990; Hall et al. 1998) and, interestingly, space flight (Fischer et al. 1972; Kimzey et al. 1976). In these studies, strong associations between stress and impaired cytotoxic T-cell activity were found.

However, very few studies have focused on purely psychological experimental stressors. Some studies have utilised psychological stress in the form of confronting subjects with a short-term uncontrollable interpersonal situation and found resulting increases in suppressor T-lymphocyte concentrations and cytotoxic T-cell activity (Naliboff et al. 1991; Brosschot et al. 1991, 1992).

Esterling et al. (1996) found a decrease in cytotoxic T-cell function in chronic caregivers as long as 3 years after the death of the care receiver, suggesting that the chronic stresses of caregiving may have far-reaching and potentially important physiological implications. They suggested that former caregivers are not reintegrated into society, which results in a persistent lack of social support which sustains the level of chronic stress even after the death of the care receiver. These results seem to indicate that the immune system response to a short-term psychological stress situation and a chronic stress situation are different and significant with regards to cytotoxic T-cell activity and function.

The cytotoxic T-cells play an important role in a variety of immune functions including defence against viral infections, surveillance of tumour cells, and most particularly the control of metastases (Herberman 1992). Some studies involving humans and their response to stress have focused on stressful life events and how the stress from these events affected the person’s risk of contracting a given disease. Epidemiological studies have demonstrated clear differences between bereaved and non-bereaved controls in terms of cancer mortality (Verbrugge 1979) and lymphocyte proliferative response and mitogenitic activity (Schleifer et al. 1983). Convergent data from several investigations suggest that bereavement may be associated with depression of some components of the immune system such as those suggested above (Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser 1991). Irwin et al. (1987a), and others (Bartrop et al. 1977; Schleifer et al. 1983; Calabrese et al. 1987) have demonstrated an impaired T-lymphocyte proliferative response in bereaved spouses that may last for a period of several months. The exact stimuli and response mechanisms of this response remain unclear (Knapp et al. 1992).

Epidemiological data also suggest that divorced individuals are at risk for both physical and mental illness (Verbrugge 1979). Evidence suggests that continued preoccupation with the ex-spouse (overattachment) leads to distress-related symptoms (Weiss 1975). Consistent with these results, Kiecolt-Glaser et al. (1988) found that divorced individuals who had been separated for shorter periods or who had stronger feelings of attachment had a decreased immune function.

Consistent data seem to be emerging that indicate a correlation between marital interruption and down-regulation of the immune system. Herbert and Cohen (1993) used meta-analytic procedures to evaluate the literature on stress and immunity in humans. In all, they examined and analysed 31 different studies and concluded that a substantial amount of evidence from both functional and enumerative measures links stress to immune function. In terms of cell numbers, they found stress is reliably associated with higher numbers of circulating white blood cells and lower numbers of circulating B cells, T cells, helper and suppressor T cells, and cytotoxic T cells. They found that stress is also reliably associated with decreases in total serum IgM and salivary IgA concentrations. Some limitations were evident with the study, namely that 11 of the 31 studies used were from the same investigators but, nevertheless, a strong correlation seemed to exist.

The effects of chronic stress were evaluated by Fiore et al. (1983) and Kiecolt-Glaser and Glaser (1991). Both groups utilised caregivers of Alzheimer’s patients as representatives of chronic stress recipients. They found that contrary to animal studies where acute stress appeared immunosuppressive and chronic stress immunoenhancing (Monjan & Collecter 1977), in humans chronic stress resulted in chronic down-regulation of the immune system. In contrast to the results obtained in studies of the effects of long-term or chronic stress, several recent studies have focused on the effects of relatively short-term stressful life events such as tandem jumps of first-time parachutists (Schedlowski et al. 1993), waiting for notification of HIV-1 test results (Ironson et al. 1990), and threatened missile attack at Israeli sites (Weiss et al. 1996). The results of these studies found an elevation rather than reduction in the number and activity of cytotoxic T cells, both immediately before and after the stressful event.

Behavioural conditioning

The paper of Ader and Cohen (1975) demonstrating the classic conditioning of immune response sparked a renewed interest in the concept of psychoneuroimmune link. In their study, Ader and Cohen used a distinctively flavoured drink, which they paired with an injection of immunosuppressive drug (cyclophosphamide) on a population of rats. When the rats, which had been conditioned to immunosuppression, stopped receiving the injection of cyclophosphamine but drank from the flavoured drink they still remained immunosuppressed. Ader and Cohen, through a series of further experiments (Ader & Cohen 1993) confirmed by other studies (Lysle et al. 1988) showed that the rat’s immune systems had been conditioned to respond through psychological conditioning.

The biological impact of such a discovery becomes evident when applications of the conditioning operation occur in tissue graph rejection experiments. In mice, re-exposure to a condition stimulus previously paired with an immunosuppressive drug treatment prolonged the survival of foreign tissue graphs on mice without further administration of the drug (Gorczynski 1990).

Further clinical implications for humans remain to be identified. However, at least one case study describes the successful application of conditioning in reducing the amount of Cytoxan therapy received by a child with lupus (Olness & Ader 1992). Another team of investigators used a different form of behavioural conditioning to investigate the effects of coping on immune system function. Laudenslager et al. (1983) used a shocking mechanism on human subjects and found that inescapable shocks lead to a suppression of lymphocyte proliferation, whereas escapable shocks did not. They concluded that the degree of control that a subject had over the unpleasant stimuli determined the type of immune response to a large degree.

Humour and happiness

The idea that humour and health may be related is not a new idea. In fact, it is an idea that has long enjoyed widespread support, both from the lay public and among professionals in the fields of psychology and medicine (Lefcourt & Martin 1986). It seems surprising that a relationship so widely held by society has not been investigated by more than a handful of researchers. Cousins (1979), in an autobiographical account, describes the positive effect that humour had on his recovery from an extremely painful disease. He reported a decrease in pain perception and a drop in his sedimentation rate following exposure to humour. Several investigators have shown that mirthful laughter is accompanied by a widespread variety of psychobiological changes. These changes include respiratory changes, facial muscle contractions, circulatory changes, sympathetic activation (Fry 1980; Lefcourt & Martin 1986), and an increase in spontaneous lymphocyte blastogenesis and natural killer cell activity (Lefcourt & Thomas 1998). Dillon et al. (1985) used a humorous videotape to induce humour, compared to a didactic videotape and found increases in salivary IgA concentration in subjects viewing the humorous videotape. Dillon and Totten (1989) expanded on Dillon’s original work with a study involving breastfeeding mothers. The study found a positive correlation between ratings on the cognitive Humour Scale and a decreased incidence of respiratory infections in both mothers and their breastfed babies. Futterman et al. (1992) induced positive and negative mental states in actors and found that all experimentally induced mood states produced greater immunological fluctuations in natural killer cells than a neutral state and that these effects were stronger for more ‘aroused’ moods such as happiness. If immune function can be experimentally conditioned in humans, then happiness may be one step on the voyage to understanding the relationship between a positive emotional state and healing.

Most theories of humour involve the concept of psychological arousal as a necessary component of humour elicitation; however, several fundamental issues remain unresolved when studying humour as an emotional concept. First, humour must be differentiated from other aesthetic qualities that may be associated with making one laugh, such as beauty, wit, nonsense, sarcasm, ridicule, satire, or irony. Confusion of results may occur when researchers consider humour as an umbrella term for all phenomena that make one laugh. This terminology involves such diverse categories as aggressive humour, copying humour, mock humour, ridicule humour, and just plain humour. It is important to appreciate that these different forms of humour may be generated by different neuronal mechanisms which may result in different effects on the other systems of the body.

image Clinical case answers

Case 16.2

Case 16.3

References

Ader R., Cohen N. Behaviourally conditioned immunosuppression. Psychosom. Med.. 1975;37:333-340.

Ader R., Cohen N. Psychoneuroimmunology: conditioning and stress. Annu. Rev. Psychol.. 1993;44:53-58.

Agras, S., Sylvester, D., Oliveau, D., 1969. The Epidemiology of Common Fears and Phobias. Unpublished manuscript.

Amsterdam J.D., Henle W., Winokur A., et al. Sermin antibodies to Ebstein-Barr virus in patients with major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1986;143:1593-1596.

Apter M.J. Reversal theory as a theory of the emotions. In: Apter M.J., Kerr J.H., Cowles M.P., editors. Progress in Reversal Theory. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1988.

Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1986.

Bandura A. Social cognitive theory. In: Vasta R., editor. Annals of Child Development. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1989:1-60.

Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In: Kirtines W.M., Gewitz J.L., editors. Handbook of Moral Behaviour and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Elbaum; 1991:45-103.

Bartrop R.W., Lazarus L., Lockhurst E., Kiloch L.G., Penny R. Depressed lymphocyte function after bereavement. Lancet. 1977;1:834-836.

Beck A.T. Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Harper & Row, 1967.

Berger P.A. Medical treatment of mental illness. Science. 1978;200:974-981.

Berk L. Child Development, third ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1994.

Black F.O., Pesznecker S.C. Vestibular adaptation and rehabilitation. Cur. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg.. 2003;11(5):355-360.

Blalock J.E. The immune system as a sensory organ. J. Immunol.. 1984;132:1067-1070.

Bondi M., Zannino L. Psychological stress, neuromodulation and susceptibility to infectious diseases in animals and man: A review. Psychother. Psychosom.. 1997;66:3-26.

Bootzin R.R., Acocella J.R. Abnormal Psychology; Current Perspectives. New York: Random House, 1984.

Boyd J.H., Weissman M.M. Epidemiology of affective disorders: A reexamination and future directions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1981;38:1039-1045.

Brosschot J., Smelt D., DeSmet M., Heijnen C.J., Olff M., Ballieux R.E.G.L.R. Effects of experimental psychological stress of T-lymphocytes and NK cells in man: an exploratory study. J. Psychophysiol.. 1991;5:59-67.

Brosschot J., Benschop R., Godaert G., deSmet M.B.M., Olff M., Heijnen C.J., Ballieux R.E. Effects of experimental psychological stress on distribution and function of peripheral blood cells. Psychosom. Med.. 1992;54:394-406.

Calabrese J.R., Kling M.A., Gold P.W. Alterations in immunocompetence during stress, bereavement, and depression: focus on neuroendocrine regulation. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1987;144(9):1123-1134.

Campos, J.J., Mumme, D.L., Kermoian, R., et al. 1994. A functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. In: Fox, N.A. (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation. monographs of the society for research in child development 59 (2 and 3), 284–303.

Cohen E., Motto J.A., Seiden R.H. An instrument for evaluating suicide potential: A preliminary study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1966;122:886-891.

Cohen S., Tyrrel D.A.J., Smith A.P. Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. N. Engl. J. Med.. 1991;325:606-612.

Cousins N. Anatomy of an Illness. New York: Norton, 1979.

Crow T.J. A re-evaluation of the viral hypothesis: Is psychosis the result of retroviral integration at a site close to the cerebral dominance gene? Br. J. Psychiatry. 1984;145:243-253.

Davidson R.J. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cogn.. 1992;20:125-151.

Dillon K.M., Minchoff B., Baker K. Positive emotional state enhancement of the immune system. Int. J. Psychiatr. Med.. 1985;15:13-17.

Dillon K.M., Totten M.C. Psychological factors immunocompetence and health of breast feeding mothers and their infants. J. Genet. Psychol.. 1989;150:155-162.

Epstein H. A sin or a right? The New York Times Magazine. 1974:91-94. September 8

Epstein S. Emotion and self theory. In: Lewis M., Haviland J., editors. Handbook of Emotion. New York: Gilford Press, 1993.

Epstein S., Lipson A., Holstein C., Huh E. Irrational reactions to negative outcomes; Evidence for two conceptual systems. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.. 1992;62:328-338.

Etzel B., Gewirtz J. Experimental modification of caretaker- maintained high rate operant crying with reinforcement of eye contact and smiling. J. Exp. Child Psychol.. 1967;5:303-317.

Evans P.D., Edgerton N. Life events and moods as predictors of the common cold. Br. J. Med. Psychol.. 1991;64:35-44.

Fawzy F.I. Behaviour and immunity. In: Kaplan H.I., Socock B.J., editors. Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1995:1559-1570.

Finch, J.R., Smith, J.P., Pokoray, A.D. 1970. Vehicular Studies. Paper presented at meetings of the American Psychiatric Association.

Fiore J., Becker J., Coppel D.B. Social network interactions: A buffer or a stress? Am. J. Community Psychol.. 1983;11:423-429.

Fischer C.L., Daniels J.C., Levin W.C., et al. Effects of the space flight environment on man’s immune system. II. Lymphocyte counts and reactivity. Aerosp. Med.. 1972;43:1122-1125.

Flor-Henry P. Observations, reflections, and speculations on the cerebral determinants of mood and on the bilateral asymmetrical distribution of the major neurotransmitter systems. Acta Neurol. Scand. Suppl.. 1986;74:75-89.

Frey K.P. About reversal theory. In: Suebak S., Apter M., editors. Stress and Health: The Reversal Theory Perspective. London: Taylor and Francis, 1997.

Fry W.F.Jr. Humour and Healing. San Francisco: University of California Press, 1980.

Futterman A.D., Kemeny M.E., Shapiro D., Polonsky W., Fahey J.L. Immunological variability associated with experimentally induced positive and negative states. Psychol. Med.. 1992;22:231-238.

Gaffan E.A., Gaffan D., Harrison S. Disconnection of the amygdala from visual association cortex impairs visual reward- association learning in monkeys. J. Neurosci.. 1988;8(9):3144-3150.

Gorczynski R.M. Conditioned enhancement of skin allographs in mice. Brain, Beha. Immun.. 1990;4:85-92.

Greene W.A. Psychology factors and retiuloendothelial disease. Psychosom. Med.. 1954;16:220-230.

Gunderson J.G., Autry J.H., Mosher L.R., Buchsbaum S. Special report: schizophrenia, 1973. Schizophr. Bull.. 1974;2:15-54.

Halgren E., LeDoux J.E. Emotional networks in the brain. In: Lewis M., Haviland J., editors. Handbook of Emotion. New York: Gilford Press, 1993.

Hall M., Baum A., Buysse D., et al. Sleep as a mediator of the stress-immune relationship. Psychosom. Med.. 1998;60:48-51.

Halonen P.E., Rimon R., Arochouka K. Antibody levels to herpes simplex type 1 measles and rubella viruses in psychiatric patients. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1974;125:461-465.

Hebb D.O. The effects of early experience on problem solving at maturity. Am. Psychol.. 1947;2:306-307.

Hebb D.O. The Organisation of Behaviour. New York: Wiley, 1949.

Herberman R. Tumor immunology. JAMA. 1992;268:2935-2939.

Herbert T.B., Cohen S. Stress and immunity in humans: a meta-analytic review. Psychosom. Med.. 1993;55:364-379.

Hirschfeld R.M.A., Cross C.K. Epidemiology of affective disorders: psychosocial risk factors. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1982;39:35-46.

Ironson G., LaPerriere A.A., Antoni M., O’Hearn P., Schneiderman N., Klimas N., Fletcher. Changes in immune and psychological measures as a function of anticipation and reaction to news of HIV-1 antibody testing. Psychosom. Med.. 1990;52:247-270.

Irwin M., Daniels M., Smith T.L., Bloom E., Weiner H. Impaired natural killer cell activity during bereavement. Brain Behav. Immun.. 1987;1:98-104.

Irwin M., Smith T.L., Gillin J.C. Low natural killer cytotoxicity in major depression. Life Sci.. 1987;41:2127-2133.

Irwin M., Caldwell C.L., Smith T.L., Brown S., Schuckit M.A., Gillin J.C. Major depressive disorder, alcoholism, reduced natural killer cell cytotoxicity. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1990;47:713-718.

Ishigami T. The influence of psychic acts on the progress of pulmonary tuberculosis. Am. Rev.Tuberc.. 1919;2:470-484.

Izard C.E. Patterns of Emotions – A New Analysis of Anxiety and Depression. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Johnstone E.C., Whaley K. Antinuclear antibodies in psychiatric illness: Their relationships to diagnosis and drug treatment. Br. Med. J.. 1975;2:724-725.

Jones E. Rationalization in everyday life. In Papers on Psychoanalysis. New York: Wood; 1963.

Kagan J., Kearsley R.B., Zelazo P.R. Infancy: Its Place in Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1978.

Kemeny M.E. Stressful events, psychological responses and progression of HIV infection. In: Glaser R., Kieholt-Glaser J.K., editors. Handbook of Human Stress and Immunity. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994:245-266.

Kiecolt-Glaser J.K., Garner W., Speicher C., Penn G.M., Holliday J., Glaser R. Psychological modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students. Psychosom. Med.. 1984;46:7-14.

Kiecolt-Glaser J.K., Ricker D., George J., Messick G., Speicher C.E., Garner W., Glaser R. Urinary cortisol levels, cellular immunocompetency and loneliness in psychiatric patients. Psychosom. Med.. 1984;46:15-23.

Kiecolt-Glaser J.K., Kennedy S., Malkoff S., Fisher L., Speicher C.E., Glaser R. Marital discord and immunity in males. Psychosom. Med.. 1988;50:213-229.

Kiecolt-Glaser J.K., Glaser R. Stress and immune function in humans. In Ader R., Felten D.L., Cohen N., editors: Psychoneuroimmunology, second ed., San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1991.

Kimzey S.L., Johnson P.C., Ritzman S.E., Johnson P.C., Ritzman S.E., Mengel C.E. Hematology and immunology studies the second manned Skylab mission. Aviat. Space Environ. Med.. 1976;47:383-390.

Kirch D.G. Infection and autoimmunity as etiological factors in schizophrenia: A review and reappraisal. Schizophr. Bull.. 1993;19:355-370.

Knapp P.H., Levy E.M., Giorgi R.G., Black P.H., Fox B.H., Heeren T.C. Short term immunological effects of induced emotion. Psychosom. Med.. 1992;54:133-148.

Koh K.B. The relationship between stress and natural killer-cell activity in medical college students. Korean J. Psychosom. Med.. 1993;3:3-10.

Koh K.B. Exam Stress Enhances Lymphocyte Proliferation. 14th World Congress of Psychosomatic Medicine, 1997.

Koh K.B. Emotion and immunity. J. Psychosom. Res.. 1998;45:107-115.

Koh K.B., Lee B.K. Reduced lymphocyte proliferation and interleukin-2 production in anxiety disorders. Psychosom. Med.. 1998;60:479-483.

Krueger R.B., Levy E.M., Cathcart E.S. Lymphocyte subsets in patients with major depression: preliminary findings. Advances. 1984;1:5-9.

Laudenslager M.L., Ryan S.M., Drugan R.C., Hyson R.L., Maier S.F. Coping and immunosuppression: Inescapable but not escapable shock suppresses lymphocyte proliferation. Science. 1983;221:568-570.

Lazarus R.S. Psychological Stress and the Coping Process. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966.

LeDoux J.E. Information flow from sensation to emotion: Plasticity in the neural computation of stimulus value. In: Gabriel M., Moore J., editors. Learning and Computational Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1990.

Lefcourt H.M., Martin R.A. Humour and Life Stress: Antidote to Adversity. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1986.

Lefcourt H.M., Thomas S. Humour and stress revisited. In: Ruch W., editor. The Sense of Humour: Explorations of a Personality Characteristic. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1998.

Levy R.M., Bredesen D.E. Controversies in HIV-related central nervous system disease: Neuropsychological aspects of HIV-1 infections. In: Volberding P., Jacobson M., editors. AIDS Clinical Review. New York: Dekker, 1989.

Linn B.S., Linn M.W., Jensen J. Anxiety and immune responsiveness. Psychol. Rep.. 1981;49:969-970.

Lyons W. An introduction to the philosophy of emotions. In: Strongman K.T., editor. International Review of Studies on Emotion. Chichester: Wiley, 1992.

Lysle D.T., Cunnick J.E., Fowler H., Rabin B.S. Pavlovian conditioning of shock induced suppression of lymphocyte reactivity, acquisition, extinction, and pre-exposure effects. Life Sci.. 1988;42:2185-2194.

McCall R., McGhee P. The discrepancy hypothesis of attention and affect. In: Weizmann F., Uzgiris I., editors. The Structure of Experience. New York: Plenum Press, 1977.

Madden K.S., Felton D.L. Experimental basis for neural-immune interactions. Physiol. Rev.. 1995;75:77-106.

May R. Contributions of existential psychotherapy. In: May R., Angel E., Ellenberger H.F., editors. Existence: A New Dimension in Psychiatry and Psychology. New York: Basic Books, 1958.

Mohl P.C., Huang L., Bowden C., Fishbach M., Vogtberger K., Tala N. Natural killer cell activity in major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1987;144:1619.

Monjan A.A., Collecter M.I. Stress induced modulation of the immune response. Science. 1977;196:307-308.

Murgatroyd S. Reversal theory and psychotherapy: A review. Couns. Psychol. Q.. 1987;3:371-381.

Naliboff B.D., Benton D., Solomon G.F., Morley J.E., Fahey J.L., Bloom E.T. Immunological changes in young and old adults during brief laboratory stress. Psychoneuroimmunology Psychosom. Med.. 1991;53:121-132.

Olness K., Ader R. Conditioning as an adjunct in the pharmacotherapy of lupus erythemotosis. J. Dev.Behav. Pediatr.. 1992;13:124-125.

Palmbald J., Cantell K., Strander H., et al. Stressor exposure and immunological response in man: Interferon – producing capacity and phagocytosis. J. Psychosom. Res.. 1976;20:193-199.

Palmbald J., Bjorn P., Wasserman J., et al. Lymphocyte and granulocyte reaction during sleep deprivation. Psychosom. Med.. 1979;41:273-278.

Persky V.M., Kempthorne-Rawson J., Shekelle R.B. Personality and risk of cancer. 20 year follow up the Western Electric Study. Psychosom. Med.. 1987;49:435-449.

Plotnikoff N., Margo A., Faith R., et al, editors. Stress and Immunity. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 1991.

Powers M., Dalgleish T. Cognition and Emotion, from Order to Disorder. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1997.

Rachman S.J., Hodgson R.R. Obsessions and Compulsions. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980.

Robins E., Gassner J., Kayes J., Wilkinson R.H., Murphy G.E. The communication of suicidal intent: A study of 134 successful (completed) suicides. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1959;1156:724-733.

Robinson R.G., Kubos K.L., Starr L. Mood disorders in stroke patients: importance of location of lesion. Brain. 1984;107:81-93.

Rolls E.T. Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amygdala. In: Aggleton J.P., editor. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley–Liss, 1992.

Rothermundt M., Arolt V., Weitzsch C., Eckhoff D., Kirchner H. Immunological dysfunction in schizophrenia: a systematic approach. Neuropsychobiology. 1998;37:186-193.

Sackeim H., Greenberg M.S., Weiman A.L. Hemispheric asymmetry in the expression of positive and negative emotions: neurologic evidence. Arch. Neurol.. 1982;39:210-218.

Schedlowski M., Jacobs R., Alkre J., et al. Psychophysiological, neuroendocrine and cellular immune reactions under psychological stress. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:87-90.

Schleifer S.J., Keller S.E., Camerino M., et al. Suppression of lymphocyte stimulation following bereavement. JAMA. 1983;250:374-377.

Schleifer S.J., Keller S.E., Bond R.N., et al. Major depressive disorder: role of age, sex, severity and hospitalization. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1989;46:81-87.

Schweitzer L. Evidence of right cerebral hemisphere dysfunction in schizophrenic patients with left hemisphere overactivation. Biol. Psychiatry. 1982;17(6):655-673.

Selye H. A syndrome produced by diverse, nocuous agents. Nature. 1936;138:132.

Seymour R. Neuroendocrine-immune interactions. N. Engl. J. Med.. 1993;329:1246-1253.

Shneidman E.S., Mandelkorn P. How to prevent suicide. In: Shneidman E.S., Farberow N.L., Litman R.E., editors. The Psychology of Suicide. New York: Science House, 1970.

Shochet B.R. Recognizing the suicidal patient. Mod. Med.. 1970;38:114-117.123.

Skinner B.F. About Behaviourism. New York: Alfred Knopf, 1974.

Stein M., Keller S.E., Schleifer S.I. Stress and immunomodulation: The role of depression and neuroendocrine function. J. Immunol.. 1985;135:827-833.

Stein M., Miller A.H., Trestman R.L. Depression and the immune system. In Ader R., Felten D.L., Cohen N., editors: Psychoneuroimmunology, second ed., San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1991.

Stengel E. Suicide and Attempted Suicide. Baltimore, MD: Penguin, 1964.

Surman O.S., Williams J., Sheehan D.V., et al. Immunological response to stress in agoraphobia and panic attacks. Biol. Psychiatry. 1986;21:768-774.

Syvalahti E. Biological factors in schizophrenia. Structural and functional aspects. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1994;164:9-14.

Verbrugge L.M. Marital status and health. J. Marriage Fam.. 1979;41:267-285.

Watson J.B., Raynor R. Conditioned emotional reactions. J. Exp. Psychol.. 1920;3:1-14.

Weiss D.W., Hirt R., Tarcic N., Berzon Y., Ben-Zur H., Breznitz S., Glaser B., Grover N. Studies in psychoneuroimmunology (PNI): psychological immunological and neuroendocrinological parameters in Israeli civilians during and after a period of SCUD missile attacks. Behav. Med.. 1996;22:5-14.

Weiss R.S. Marital Separation. New York: Basic Books, 1975.

Weisse C.S., Pato C.N., McAllister C.G., et al. Differential effects of controllable and uncontrollable acute stress on lymphocyte proliferation and leukocyte percentages in humans. Brain Behav. Immun.. 1990;4:339-351.

Woodruff R.A., Clayton P.J., Guze S.B. Is everyone depressed? Am. J. Psychiatry. 1975;132:627-628.