HIGHER VISUOPERCEPTUAL DISORDERS AND DISORDERS OF SPATIAL COGNITION

Published on 10/04/2015 by admin

Filed under Neurology

Last modified 10/04/2015

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 1710 times

CHAPTER 5 HIGHER VISUOPERCEPTUAL DISORDERS AND DISORDERS OF SPATIAL COGNITION

At least one third of the primate brain is devoted to visual perception and visual processing.1 A vast amount has been learned about the visual system in the past 100 years or so, from both laboratory studies and studies of clinical and healthy populations. This research has identified multiple areas in the cerebral cortex (not just the occipital lobe) that receive visual input (Figs. 5-1 through 5-3). There is both parallel and distributed processing of visual stimuli. One of the most famous depictions are the diagrams of Van Essen and colleagues,2 which map the macaque cerebral cortex (Fig. 5-1).

image

Figure 5-2 Hierarchy of visual areas in the macaque. This diagram shows over 30 visual cortical areas, shaded similarly to Figure 5-1, two subcortical visual stages—the retinal ganglion cell layer (RGC) and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), plus several non-visual areas such as area 7b of the somatosensory cortex, area 36 and the hippocampal complex (HC). These areas are connected by extensive linkages, most of which have been demonstrated to be reciprocal pathways. The patterns of connections help to illustrate two fundamental principles of organization: hierarchical connections and parallel processing.

(From Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC: Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1991; 1:1-47.)

The term higher visuoperceptual disorders carries the implication of abnormalities at the level of the thalamus and above, with the majority of cases arising from diseases of the cerebral hemispheres.

Many disorders of higher visuoperceptual function have been described, some of them in syndromic fashion, and include Balint’s syndrome, cerebral akinetopsia (rare), cerebral dyschromatopsia (somewhat more common), disorders of face recognition (prosopagnosia), environmental disorientation (which overlaps with spatial disorientation: see also Chapter 6), and visual agnosias. Even one of the rarer language disorders may be thought of as a primary visual disturbance: alexia without agraphia (see Chapter 3). Higher-order visual complaints are often one of the first indications of a number of other neurological illnesses such as prion diseases.

All these entities are of interest in their own right and may lead to significant morbidity. They are commonly not initially recognized, which causes further distress and difficulty for the patients, their families, and those who interact with them on a daily basis, such as work colleagues and health professionals.

THE TWO-SYSTEMS APPROACH TO HIGHER VISUOSPATIAL FUNCTIONING

Higher visual processing attempts to achieve two primary objectives: the identification of visual stimuli and their localization in space. These two goals of visual analysis are often abbreviated as “what” and “where” (Fig. 5-4). Research has demonstrated that these two goals are achieved relatively independently through two anatomically separated systems known as the ventral (what) and dorsal (where) visual processing pathways.68 The fundamental concept is that there is a dorsal stream of information concerned with an object’s location and a ventral stream concerned with its identity. Because this distinction is so widespread in the literature, it must be considered, but even the proponents of this dualistic model freely admit that it is an oversimplification of the true situation, given that strong interactions occur between the two networks.

We use this dichotomy in the current chapter to help distinguish higher visual disorders that are primarily deficits of visuoperception from those that are largely visuospatial in nature.

ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY

In general, higher visuoperceptual and visuospatial disorders reflect damage to one or several multiple brain regions including (but not restricted to) the occipital lobes, temporal lobes, parietal lobes, and underlying white matter. In many of the described syndromes, the right hemisphere appears to be more affected than the left, with many investigators proposing that much of the bilateral activation on higher visual tasks results from the transcallosal influence of the right hemisphere.9,10 The precise nature of the damage for each particular disorder is discussed in the following sections.

Disorders of the Temporal Vision–Related Cortex (Ventral Processing Stream)

Damage to the cortical regions of the ventral processing stream results in a variety of disturbances of visual form and color perception (Table 5-1). These can include dyschromatopsia and alexia without agraphia. Most commonly, however, damage to these regions results in a form of visual agnosia, a clinical syndrome characterized by an inability to recognize a visually presented object despite the presence of adequate cognition, visual acuity, attention, and language skills.11,12

TABLE 5-1 Summary of Clinical Presentation and Location of Lesion in the Temporo-Occipital Vision Disorders

Condition Description Location of Lesion
Dyschromatopsia Inability to distinguish colors by hue Inferior occipital lobe, occipitotemporal lobe
Color agnosia (and anomia) Inability to name colors despite being able to match and sort items by color
Alexia without agraphia or pure alexia Inability to read despite preserved ability to write Occipitotemporal lobes
Apperceptive (object) Inability to recognize familiar objects despite intact visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and often color perception and stereopsis Bilateral occiptotemporal lobes
Associative agnosia Sensory percept stripped of meaning Bilateral occiptotemporal lobes
Prosopagnosia Inability to recognize familiar faces Bilateral (but right > left) occipitotemporal lobes
Environmental agnosia Inability to recognize familiar environments Right occipitotemporal lobes

Agnosias are commonly subdivided on the basis of a distinction made by Lissauer,13 who posited in 1890 that the process of recognition has two distinct stages: apperception and association. Apperception is the ability to form a conscious percept of a sensory impression (e.g., an object), which can be thought of as the construction of different visual attributes of the stimulus into a whole percept. Patients who present with these difficulties are said to have the syndrome of apperceptive agnosia, sometimes referred to as apperceptive visual object agnosia.12 Association, on the other hand, refers to the imparting of meaning to the percept, achieved through the use of matching or linking the percept to a previous experience or knowledge.13 Patients with association difficulties, who are said to have the syndrome of associative visual agnosia, often have difficulty accessing the memory of an object’s name or its meaning from the visual stimulus, despite a more or less correctly perceived visual percept, and demonstrably intact knowledge (if accessed via other modalities) of the object’s name and semantic attributes.

Visual agnosias are often further classified on the basis of the categorical impairment such as prosopagnosia (agnosia for faces), environmental agnosia (agnosia for topographical orientation), and color agnosia.

Associative Agnosia

Those patients with associative agnosia have apparently preserved sensory capability and may retain the ability to match and copy common objects but still fail to recognize them. Such cases led Teuber19 in 1968 to describe this as “a normal percept that has somehow been stripped of its meaning.” Some patients may have their problem limited to a specific class of objects, of which the best known is prosopagnosia—the failure to discriminate and recognize faces. Other object classes with which specific difficulties have been reported include animals or animal species, plants, foods, clothing, makes of cars, colors, or places. Some examples are discussed in the following paragraphs.

The cerebral lesions are in areas similar to those causing apperceptive agnosia but have occasionally been linked to more diffuse cerebral damage such as hypoxic/ischemic brain damage or carbon monoxide poisoning and progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy.