The Visual System

Published on 16/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Basic Science

Last modified 22/04/2025

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 1633 times

17 The Visual System

The visual system is the most studied sensory system, partly because we are such a visually oriented species and partly because of its relative simplicity. In addition, the visual pathway is highly organized in a topographical sense, so even though it stretches from the front of your face to the back of your head, damage anyplace causes deficits that are relatively easy to understand.

The Eye Has Three Concentric Tissue Layers and a Lens

Vertebrate eyes perform functions analogous to those performed by cameras, but do so using three roughly spherical, concentric tissue layers either derived from or comparable to the dura mater, the pia-arachnoid, and the CNS (Fig. 17-1). The thick, collagenous outer layer forms the sclera—the white of the eye—and continues anteriorly as the cornea and posteriorly as the dural optic nerve sheath. The middle layer is loose, vascular connective tissue that forms the pigmented choroid that lines the sclera; it continues anteriorly as the vascular core of the ciliary body, the ciliary muscle, and most of the iris. The innermost layer, itself a double layer because of the way the eye develops (THB6 Figure 17-1, p. 416), forms the neural retina (closer to the interior of the eye) and the retinal pigment epithelium (adjacent to the choroid); it continues anteriorly as the double-layered epithelial covering of the ciliary body and the posterior surface of the iris. Suspended inside the eye, and not really part of any of these tissue layers, is the lens.

Collectively, structures derived from these three layers, together with the lens, take care of the functions dealt with by cameras: keeping a photosensitive surface in a stable position, focusing images of objects at different distances onto this surface, regulating the amount of light that reaches the photosensitive surface, and absorbing stray light.

The Retina Contains Five Major Neuronal Cell Types

The job of the retina is to convert patterns of light into trains of action potentials in the optic nerve. It does this using five basic cell types (Fig. 17-2), whose cell bodies are arranged in three layers (outer and inner nuclear layers, ganglion cell layer). Alternating with these three layers of cell bodies are an outer and an inner plexiform layer where the synaptic interactions occur. In the outer plexiform layer, photoreceptor cells (rods and cones) bring visual information in, bipolar cells take it out, and horizontal cells mediate lateral interactions. In the inner plexiform layer bipolar cells bring visual information in, ganglion cells take it out (their axons form the optic nerve), and amacrine cells mediate lateral interactions.

Standard descriptions of the retina as a 10-layered structure also include a row of junctions between adjacent photoreceptors (outer limiting membrane), the layer of ganglion cell axons (nerve fiber layer), and the basal lamina on the vitreal surface of the retina (inner limiting membrane). Oddly enough, the layers are arranged so that the last part of vertebrate neural retinas reached by light is the photosensitive parts of the rod and cone cells, embedded in processes of pigment epithelial cells.

The Retina Is Regionally Specialized

Ganglion cell axons travel along the vitreal surface of the retina, and so they need to pierce the sclera to leave the eye in the optic nerve. They do so by converging on the optic disk, slightly medial to the optic axis, turning 90° posteriorly, and leaving the eye. Here the optic nerve acquires a dural (scleral) sheath continuous with the dural covering of the CNS. (The dural sheath is lined with arachnoid and contains a bit of subarachnoid space; increases in intracranial pressure are therefore transmitted along the optic nerve and cause papilledema, or swelling of the optic disk.) Because there are no photoreceptors at the optic disk, it corresponds to a blind spot in the visual field of each eye. The optics of the eye reverse images on the retina, so the blind spot of each eye lies near the horizontal meridian of the visual field, slightly lateral to the center of the field.

The center of the visual field corresponds to the fovea, a small retinal region in the middle of a pigmented zone called the macula. The fovea is filled with thin, densely packed cones and no rods. All the other neuronal types are pushed toward the periphery, so the center of the fovea is a small pit (THB6 Figure 17-11, p. 425). Outside the fovea, the number of cones diminishes quickly. The packing density of rods, in contrast, first increases rapidly and then declines slowly. We have three different types of cones in terms of the wavelength to which each is most sensitive, so the total cone population can be used for color vision. Rods, on the other hand, come in only one variety but function at lower light levels than do cones. The fovea, with its densely packed cones, is therefore specialized for high spatial acuity and color vision, but only at moderate or high levels of illumination. The region around the fovea, with many rods and few cones, has reasonably good spatial acuity, works at low light levels, but is not very useful for color vision. Finally, the peripheral retina, with few rods and even fewer cones, is mostly good for telling us that something is moving around out there.

Retinal Neurons Translate Patterns of Light into Patterns of Contrast

The retina, like camera film or the sensor in a digital camera, is photosensitive, but that’s about as far as the similarity goes. Visual systems are designed to make things look the same to us whether they’re up close or far away, dimly lit or brightly lit, at sunrise or midday. This is accomplished by a process that begins in the retina: Single optic nerve fibers report not so much on the actual illumination at some location in the visual field as on differences between the illumination at one location and neighboring locations. The result is that something like the edge of a letter looks pretty much the same whether we look at it in room light or sunlight.

Ganglion Cells Have Center-Surround Receptive Fields

Ganglion cells, as mentioned previously, are contrast detectors. The receptive field of each has a center and a surround. The center is a central spot where light causes the cell to fire faster (ON-center) or slower (OFF-center), and the surround is an area where light has just the opposite effect. The result is that if uniform illumination (the level doesn’t matter) covers the whole receptive field, the center and surround more or less cancel each other out. In contrast, if the illumination is nonuniform, either the center or the surround will “win” and the ganglion cell will signal the difference between the two.

The centers of ganglion cell receptive fields result from “straight-through” transmission from receptors to bipolar cells to ganglion cells (Fig. 17-4). The surrounds result from lateral interactions mediated by horizontal and amacrine cells (especially horizontal cells).

Half of the Visual Field of Each Eye Is Mapped Systematically in the Contralateral Cerebral Hemisphere

The central visual pathway has two important anatomical tasks, one related to crossings of the midline and the other to maps.

Because our eyes face forward their visual fields overlap to a great extent (THB6 Figure 17-32, p. 445), so it would make sense for information from each retina about the contralateral half of the visual world to reach a given side of the brain. This is neatly taken care of by a partial decussation of the optic nerves in the optic chiasm, in which the ganglion cell axons from the nasal half of each retina cross the midline and join undecussated fibers from the temporal half of the other retina in the optic tract. (For example, because the optics of the eye reverse things, the temporal half of the left retina and the nasal half of the right retina both “look at” the right half of the visual world.) This separation of the visual world into two halves is maintained in the rest of the visual pathway. The optic tract ends in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus. The lateral geniculate gives rise to the optic radiation, which passes through the retrolenticular and sublenticular parts of the internal capsule and ends in primary visual cortex above and below the calcarine sulcus.

As in the case of other sensory systems, the visual system maintains an orderly map of the information it carries and emphasizes in this map certain functionally important regions. In this case, the visual pathway maintains a retinotopic map of the image falling on each retina, with a disproportionately large number of fibers representing the fovea. The mapping culminates in primary visual cortex, where the retina is represented right side up (i.e., superior fields below the calcarine sulcus, inferior fields above), the fovea posteriorly at the occipital pole and the periphery anteriorly (THB6 Figure 17-29, p. 443). The foveal representation, relative to the size of the fovea, is much larger than the representation of the periphery.

Primary Visual Cortex Sorts Visual Information and Distributes It to Other Cortical Areas

An important strategy used by the brain in information processing is to pick something apart into subcomponents and work on these separately. In visual processing this means dissecting the patterns of illumination coming from different parts of the visual field into their component elements—color, movement, borders, etc. This process begins way back in the retina, where small (and numerous) ganglion cells are particularly sensitive to colors and edges but other, larger ganglion cells are more sensitive to movement.

Early Experience Has Permanent Effects on the Visual System

The basic wiring pattern of the visual system is genetically determined and present at birth. However, there is a period of plasticity early in life during which visual experience is critical for refining and even maintaining these connections (see Chapter 24). Anything that interferes with normal binocular vision during this period (e.g., cataracts, misalignment of the eyes) can cause permanent changes in connections and permanent visual deficits. The duration of the critical period of plasticity varies from cortical area to area and from species to species, but may be as long as several years in humans.

Reflex Circuits Adjust the Size of the Pupil and the Focal Length of the Lens

The eye has its own equivalent of autoexposure and autofocus systems, based on muscles and reflex connections rather than photodiodes and motors.

Illumination of Either Retina Causes Both Pupils to Constrict

The size of the pupil is determined by the balance between a relatively strong sphincter and a relatively weak dilator (Fig. 17-7). The sphincter receives parasympathetic innervation via the oculomotor nerve and the ciliary ganglion and is normally activated during the pupillary light reflex and the near reflex (next section). The dilator receives sympathetic innervation via the intermediolateral cell column of the spinal cord and the superior cervical ganglion. The preganglionic sympathetic neurons for the dilator can be activated by long descending pathways from the ipsilateral half of the hypothalamus, as well as by other routes.

Pupils of significantly unequal size usually signify damage to some aspect of the autonomic innervation of one eye or to the iris itself. A dilated pupil (mydriasis), unresponsive to all stimuli, could be caused by damage to the ipsilateral oculomotor nerve. Such damage, if it affected the entire nerve, would also be accompanied by weakness of the other muscles supplied by the third nerve, most prominently resulting in ptosis (because of a weak levator palpebrae) and lateral strabismus (because of an unopposed lateral rectus). A pupil that was relatively constricted (miosis), but still responsive to light shone through it, could be caused by damage to either its preganglionic or its postganglionic sympathetic innervation, or to fibers on the ipsilateral side of the brainstem as they descend from the hypothalamus to the spinal cord. (In the pons and medulla, the latter fibers are located near the spinothalamic tract.) This would constitute part of Horner’s syndrome and would be accompanied by slight ipsilateral ptosis (weakness of the sympathetically innervated tarsal muscles) but not by any weakness of other extraocular muscles.

A commonly tested cranial nerve reflex is the pupillary light reflex (Fig. 17-8). Light shone through one pupil causes both sphincters to contract equally. The response of the illuminated eye is the direct reflex, and the equal response of the unilluminated eye is the consensual reflex. Afferent impulses for this reflex arc travel along ganglion cell axons in the optic nerve; half of these cross in the optic chiasm. However, they bypass the lateral geniculate nucleus and travel instead through the brachium of the superior colliculus to the pretectal area, just rostral to the superior colliculus at the midbrain-diencephalon junction. Fibers from the pretectal area then distribute bilaterally to the Edinger-Westphal subnucleus of the oculomotor nucleus, where the preganglionic parasympathetic neurons live. Because of the bilateral distribution of fibers both in the optic chiasm and in going from each pretectal area to the oculomotor nuclei, light in one eye causes equal constriction of both pupils. Optic nerve damage produces equal pupils, neither one of which responds to light shone into the eye ipsilateral to the damage, but both of which respond normally to light shone into the contralateral eye. Oculomotor nerve damage, in contrast, causes a dilated ipsilateral pupil that does not respond to light shone into either eye.

Study Questions

For questions 1-6 choose the lesion site (A-M) most likely to account for the visual field deficits indicated on the left (black areas indicate defective parts of the field).