THE NEUROLOGICAL HISTORY

Published on 03/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Neurology

Last modified 03/03/2015

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 1170 times

chapter 2

The Neurological History

This chapter will endeavour to provide a basic understanding of the principles of history taking so that the time spent on the ward learning how to take a history can be more effective, more interesting and more enjoyable. In particular, the pivotal aspects of clinical neurology will be discussed.

Almost invariably the difference between making the correct diagnosis and not is a direct consequence of the time spent taking a very detailed history. The most common mistake that inexperienced clinicians make when taking a history is that they elicit the nature of the symptoms but do not clarify the exact distribution or how the symptoms evolved. Migraine, subarachnoid haemorrhage, meningitis and a hangover will all present with headache, nausea, vomiting and photophobia. It is not the symptoms that indicate the likely diagnosis but rather the way in which those symptoms have evolved.

Using the techniques described in this chapter, there is no reason why an inexperienced student or hospital medical officer cannot obtain the same history as an experienced clinician. There is always an experienced clinician on the other end of a telephone who can be more helpful if given a good history.

Consider Cases 2.1 and 2.2.

In both of these cases the referring physician has used the nature of the symptoms, the age of the patient and the past and family history to make a diagnosis. In the first case the clinician has assumed that patients of this age are likely to suffer from a stroke and that the presence of multiple risk factors for cerebral vascular disease means that the patient’s symptoms are due to cerebral ischaemia. In fact this patient’s symptoms evolved over several weeks and were related to a malignant tumour. In the second case the past

and family history of migraine was used to make a diagnosis of migraine. This patient’s symptoms were of instantaneous onset and were related to a ruptured berry aneurysm causing subarachnoid haemorrhage (bleeding into the subarachnoid space – the space beneath the outer lining of the brain, referred to as the dura mater, and the surface of the brain).

A past history of a medical problem does not preclude the patient suffering from

a totally unrelated illness. The approach of using the past history to influence the diagnosis of the current problem often leads to a delay in diagnosing new problems in patients with chronic diseases. In most instances ‘prior probability’ is correct, and this can lull clinicians into thinking that they are clever when in fact it was difficult for them to be wrong even if they did not obtain a detailed history. Typical examples of this are an older person with a hemiparesis with the most likely cause being a stroke and the general practitioner (GP) who diagnoses most patients as having a tension-type headache and is correct the vast majority of the time simply because most patients attending a GP with headache do in fact suffer from tension-type headache.

The age of the patient and the past, family and social history simply increase the likelihood of a particular illness being present. This information should be regarded as ‘circumstantial evidence’. As in law, where many innocent people have been found guilty on the basis of circumstantial evidence, in medicine many incorrect diagnoses have been made using the age of the patient and the past, family, social and medication history to make a diagnosis.

Spencer [1] stated that it is not possible to learn how to take an effective history from a textbook. The clinical environment is the only setting in which the skills of history taking, physical examination, clinical reasoning, decision making, empathy and professionalism can be taught and learnt as an integrated whole. Although this is correct, one could use the analogy of learning to play golf: you can either hack away for years to teach yourself or you can have lessons that will make the practice more effective.

Traditionally, students have been taught to ask a long list of neurological questions about headache, dizziness, weakness, numbness etc in the forlorn hope that, after this ‘fishing expedition’ (also referred to as ‘looking for the pony’1) [2], a diagnosis will be apparent. In other words, in the hope of finding the diagnosis (‘the pony’), one collects a large list of answers to numerous specific questions that largely relate to the nature of the symptoms with little understanding about what the answers represent. It has been suggested that students should be taught to take a history this way and then taught not to take it that way. I disagree. I would like to recommentd that students should be taught how to take a history in a way that is meaningful to them rather than just collecting useless information they do not know how to use.

PRINCIPLES OF NEUROLOGICAL HISTORY TAKING

When obtaining the history from a patient with a neurological problem it is important to establish whether the problem is:

An alternative method of obtaining the history in patients presenting with intermittent disturbances of neurological function or recurrent episodes, e.g. epilepsy or headache, is recommended. In essence, with intermittent disturbances it is better to obtain a blow-by-blow description of several individual episodes. This alternative approach is described in Chapter 7, ‘Episodic disturbances of neurological function’ and Chapter 8, ‘Seizures and epilepsy’.

Having established that the problem is a monophasic illness, the three basic principles of history taking are simple:

Difficulties making a diagnosis are almost always the result of failure to establish the mode of onset, progression and duration of symptoms.

Some patients are not capable of giving an accurate history. This is often (but not always) due to the presence of an inability to speak the language, dementia, confusion or amnesia. In these circumstances it is necessary to question a relative or an eyewitness. If that is not possible, one has no choice but to use circumstantial evidence (the past and family history) and prior probability (certain conditions affect particular people of a certain sex and age) to come to a likely diagnosis.

THE UNDERLYING PATHOLOGICAL PROCESS: MODE OF ONSET, DURATION AND PROGRESSION OF SYMPTOMS

Different pathological processes evolve over variable periods of time and it is this variability that can be used to determine the most likely pathology. Three modes of onset can be loosely defined: sudden onset within seconds to minutes, gradual onset over hours to days and a very gradual onset over months to years (see Figure 2.1). The expression ‘mode of onset’ refers to the time for all symptoms to FULLY develop in terms of either their intensity and/or their distribution.

The pathological processes that reflect these modes of onset are:

Instantaneous – seconds, rarely minutes

Subacute – hours to days

Chronic – months to years

The majority of diseases that clinicians encounter fit into one of these three basic patterns, but it is important to remember that they are only a guide and there are always exceptions to the rule.

Exceptions to the rule include:

• Symptoms such as diplopia (double vision) and vertigo will, as a result of their very nature, always begin abruptly and need not necessarily indicate a pathological process with a vascular or mechanical basis (see the section ‘The nature and distribution of symptoms’).

• At time, neoplastic disorders can present with a very rapid evolution of symptoms as a result of a seizure and the rapid onset of a focal neurological deficit after the seizure; tumours can also appear to evolve more rapidly when there is a superimposed mechanical shift due to the mass effect.

• Vascular disorders can have a slower onset: for example, the stroke-in-evolution that may or may not be stepwise; the intracerebral haemorrhage that evolves over many minutes; the subdural that develops over hours to days (although here the bleeding results in a mass lesion); or the giant aneurysm that behaves as a tumour with symptoms due to the mass.

• In older patients with a chronic disease the deterioration is so slow that it is assumed that their increasing disability is simply related to old age until there is a sudden crisis, such as a fall where they are unable to get up. Patients present under these circumstances with an apparent illness of sudden onset, where in reality it is simply the ‘straw that broke the camel’s back’ phenomenon. The true nature is only clarified when a careful and detailed history of slowly evolving disability is obtained.

THE NATURE AND DISTRIBUTION OF SYMPTOMS

Buy Membership for Neurology Category to continue reading. Learn more here