Chapter 16 Pain management in neurological rehabilitation
Introduction
Neuropathic pain is defined as ‘Pain arising as a direct consequence of a lesion or disease affecting the somatosensory system’ (Treede et al., 2008). It is commonly seen following specific trauma to a nerve following injury or surgery but this chapter will focus mainly on the pain which develops as a result of neurological conditions. However, as the pathophysiology is often common between trauma-induced pain and that from disease processes, literature from a cross section will be reviewed and included.
Central or peripheral?
It should be remembered that the patient’s report of pain, the nature and severity, does not always distinguish between central and peripheral neuropathic pain; to the patient it feels the same irrespective of the location. Some tools can be used to distinguish neuropathic pain from non-neuropathic pain, which have usually been developed and tested on patients with peripheral neuropathic pain (Bennett et al., 2007) and have not been established in patients with central neuropathic pain with the exception of multiple sclerosis (MS) (Rog et al., 2007). Careful examination, quantitative testing and response to prior treatments can help elucidate the possible cause.
It is perhaps useful to refresh some of the definitions which are important in neuropathic pain (Merskey & Bogduk, 1994):
Central neuropathic pain
Central neuropathic pain can arise from primary injury or dysfunction within the central nervous system (CNS) (Merskey & Bogduk, 1994) and can arise at any level or even from more than one level. The recently suggested prerequisites for a diagnosis of the condition are:
Central neuropathic pain states can be usefully classified into three main groups: (1) pain associated with progressive neurological conditions, e.g. MS; (2) pain following stroke; and (3) pain following spinal cord injury. Another group which does not necessarily fall into these is neuropathic pain associated with HIV infection. This may be due to damage caused by the HIV virus itself or by neuropathy as a result of the antiretroviral treatment (Cox & Rice, 2008). This last group will not be considered here.
The prevalence of pain for all causes in MS has been estimated at between 43% and 70% (Moulin et al., 1988; Solaro et al., 2004). One of the best estimates for the presence of central neuropathic pain is that over 27% of patients report the condition (Osterberg et al., 2005). It is commonly widespread with an increased prevalence in the lower limbs and a variable clinical picture but a low report of paroxysmal pain. A small proportion of patients report pain at the onset of their MS but in general the incidence for central neuropathic pain syndrome (CNPS) is reported to be higher in the early years. However, this may be an artefact of diagnosis, as additional musculoskeletal pain and pain associated with spasticity may predominate in the later stages of the disease masking the true prevalence of neuropathic pain. The actual cause of CNPS in MS is difficult to determine owing to the disseminated nature of the disease. Using MRI, Osterberg et al. (2005) demonstrated hyperintensity of activity in the lateral and medial thalamic regions in one-third of MS patients with CNPS and concluded that, although there is an indication that the cause of the pain may share some similarities with central stroke pain, they could not conclude that lesions in the thalamus were the cause of the pain and postulated that lesions in the spinal cord, particularly the spino- and quintothalamic pathways, are likely to be the cause. A previous hypothesis also suggested the importance of lesions in the neospinothalamic pathway, which may become hyperexcitable following lesioning.
Pain is also seen in Parkinson’s disease but the exact reason for this is yet to be determined. It is suspected that the basal ganglia and dopaminergic systems are involved in the processing of nociception to higher centres (see Ch. 6).
Post-stroke pain is the commonest CNPS seen in the population because of the common occurrence of stroke. Andersen et al. (1995) followed 207 new stroke patients who survived for 6 months and were able to participate in a quantitative sensory testing protocol. In this study abnormal sensory signs were common (47%) and 8% of patients were diagnosed with post-stroke pain. There does not appear to be a higher prevalence in either ischaemic or haemorrhagic strokes, but because more people sustain ischaemic strokes CNPS is more commonly seen in this group of patients in clinical practice (Andersen et al., 1995).
The original description of a possible cause of post-stroke pain was made over one hundred years ago (Dejerine & Roussy, 1906) and since that time the role of lesions of the thalamus in ‘thalamic pain’ has been well documented. The literature on the importance of lesions in and around the thalamus has grown. Lateral medullary infarctions are more likely to result in damage to the spinothalamic and trigeminothalamic pathways and have the highest incidence in the development of CNPS (MacGowan et al., 1997). The incidence of pain following damage to the thalamus is also high. In an MRI study of people with post-stroke pain a high proportion of them had thalamic lesions (>60%), but multiple lesions were seen in almost all of the patients and no thalamic involvement was demonstrated in others, so the specificity of the location is difficult to demonstrate (Bowsher et al., 1998).
Pain following spinal cord injury
About two-thirds or more of people who sustain a spinal cord injury report persistent pain, but this can be due to a number of reasons, not all of which are due to CNPS (Siddall et al., 2002). A proposed classification for spinal cord injury pain describes two broad groups: nociceptive and neuropathic. Nociceptive is further broken down into musculoskeletal and visceral (Siddall et al., 2002). Neuropathic is subdivided into:
In a 5-year follow-up of 73 patients with spinal cord injury Siddall et al. found 81% of patients reported pain; of these 41% had neuropathic pain at the level of the lesion and 34% had neuropathic pain below the level of lesion. Most patients reported more than one type of pain with musculoskeletal pain being the commonest (59%), although the least severe (Siddall et al., 2003).
Mechanism of neuropathic pain
The physiological changes following a nerve injury which lead to neuropathic pain have been grouped into peripheral and central phenomena (Wallace & Rice, 2008).
Central physiological changes
Following injury, Wallarian degeneration occurs in myelinated neurones (Wallace & Rice, 2008). Activity or spontaneous activity in these may result in depolarization of adjacent uninjured unmyelinated neurones. Should this occur in mechanoreceptors, for example, stimulation of these through touch or movement may result in allodynia and hyperalgesia.
Key to the pathophysiology of neuropathic pain are the changes which take place in the ion channels of the nerve cell following nerve damage (Wallace & Rice, 2008). Perhaps the most important, or at least the best understood, is the alteration in the activation of sodium channels which alter the action potential of the cell membrane (Wallace & Rice, 2008). Much of the effectiveness of pharmacological treatment is attributed to the action of the drugs in producing sodium channel blockade (e.g. lidocaine, carbamazepine, tricyclic antidepressants). There are many different sodium channels in the DRG and the relevance of all is not understood. Some appear to increase in activity, whereas others reduce; some increase in number and others appear to change location on the cell. There are two main groups of channels classified by their sensitivity to tetrodoxin (TTX), a neurotoxin found in fish which binds to some voltage-gated channels; receptors are classified as sensitive (TTX-s) or resistant (TTX-r). TTX-s channels are found predominantly on A fibres, whereas TTX-r are seen specifically in the smaller C-fibre nociceptive neurones.
Treatment of neuropathic pain
Recent reviews on the management of both central and peripheral neuropathic pain have reached a good level of consensus guidelines for the pharmacological management of neuropathic pain (Dworkin et al., 2007) and it is worthwhile considering the recent National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) guidelines for the pharmacological management of neuropathic pain (NICE, 2010).
NICE guidelines on pharmacological therapies for neuropathic pain
Recent recommendations regarding the use of pharmacological agents in the management of neuropathic pain have been developed by NICE (2010). They describe three lines of possible pharmacological treatment for people with neuropathic pain. Patients with diabetic neuropathy should be treated specifically with duloxetine. It recommends all neuropathic pain from all other causes should be treated according to the guidelines.
Injectable drugs
Sustained infusions into the blood stream (parenteral therapy) or directly into the space surrounding the spinal cord (intrathecal injections) have been performed with positive effects on central stroke pain. Parenteral injections of lidocaineanticonvulsants, the N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) antagonist ketamine (Eide et al., 1995) and the opioid alfentanil (Eide et al., 1995) have all been demonstrated to be effective in randomized controlled trials. Intrathecal injections of morphine combined with clonidine have been demonstrated as useful in combination but not alone. The anti-spasticity drug Baclofen, a derivative of gamma-aminobutyric acid, has been demonstrated to provide pain relief, but this may be mainly due to its effect on musculoskeletal pain (Loubser & Akman, 1996), although one RCT has demonstrated a central effect on neuropathic pain (Herman et al., 1992).
Mirror box therapy
For patients with chronic regional pain syndrome of a single limb, rehabilitation using the unaffected limb reflected in a mirror to ‘trick’ the brain into seeing the limb moving without pain has been developed (Moseley et al., 2008). Patients with neuropathic pain frequently experience severe pain on movement and tactile stimulation of the affected limb. Mirror box therapy allows the patient to observe the impression that the affected limb is moving or being touched. The mechanism for this remains speculative but seeing the limb being touched activates the visual or visuotactile cells in the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) which facilitates SI neurones and promotes inhibition, and probably facilitates inhibition at the thalamic level. Giving a sense of normalization of sensory input may promote neural reorganization which leads to an improvement in tactile acuity and a reduction in pain (Moseley et al., 2008).
A recent review concluded that there was good evidence to support the use of mirror box therapy combined with a graded motor imagery programme and recommended its use be included in future guidelines (Daly & Bialocerkowski, 2009) for chronic regional pain syndrome. There is little evidence thus far to support its use in central neuropathic conditions. One study on a limited number of subjects with pain following spinal cord injury demonstrated that using a mirror and back projection of walking legs to give and impression the patient was walking led to a reduction in pain on a single exposure which was greater than imagined activity or watching a film. A further mean reduction in pain was observed over 3 weeks of daily exposure to the visual illusion of walking. However, this study used only five patients and one had to withdraw due to distress on the first exposure to the visual illusion (Moseley, 2007).
Cognitive behavioural therapy
Pain management programmes base on the principles of cognitive behavioural therapy are one of the mainstays of the management of chronic intractable pain. There are only limited data on the role of pain management programmes in the management of pain in neurological conditions. Traditionally pain management programmes run with a heterogeneous group of patients, the assumption being that the difficulties faced by people withchronic pain are common irrespective of the cause or nature of their condition. This has been questioned recently and there do appear to be subtle differences in the way in which people with a neuropathic pain problem deal with their pain when compared to those with low back pain, the commonest reason for referral to pain management (Daniel et al., 2008). The most notable difference is that those with neuropathic pain are more likely to report their pain is as a result of damage. Pain management programmes often try to avoid the term damage as it is unhelpful and focus on causes of pain that do not focus on tissue damage. However, reference to the role of previous damage as a cause of the onset of pain and to explain the continuance of pain as a consequence of damage in the past rather than ongoing injury might be useful in explaining pain to those with neuropathic pain.
The main aims of a pain management approach (Watson, 2004) are to:
The main outcomes of a pain management programme are:
Note that a reduction in pain is not listed above as an outcome. However, a reduction in pain is reported in the literature as a common outcome (Eccleston et al., 2009; Morley et al., 2008), although the effects are modest and not as large as the effects on mood.
Pain management requires a full biopsychosocial assessment of the patient. This will include a physical examination to guide any specific physical exercises required to address specific deconditioning and dysfunction as part of the programme. The patient’s understanding of their condition and their beliefs about it and how this affects their current behaviour is assessed. The current coping strategies are identified to help elucidate both helpful and unhelpful behaviours. The role of significant others is also assessed. For a full account of the assessment at management of chronic pain see specific texts such as Main et al. (2008).
Goal setting and pacing
Resumption of physical activity is a mainstay of pain management programmes, but the effects attributed to physical activity alone are very modest and recent authors have suggested a more systematized approach of activity scheduling (Main et al., 2008; Sullivan, 2008). Appropriatescheduling of activity has been demonstrated to result in reductions in fear of pain, disability and depression, although none of the data relates specifically to patients with pain from neurological conditions. It involves identifying avoided activities, setting a baseline for participation (time and intensity) and allocating time during the day to perform the activity. These activities are typically activities of daily living rather than just specific physical exercises. The patient only discontinues the activity after the agreed goal has been completed regardless of the level of pain. If the goals are chosen well it should be unlikely that the patient will discontinue the activity due to intolerable pain.
The goals are chosen so that the patient participates in desired and valued activities. Setting and achieving goals in valued activities helps to mitigate the effect of interference of pain on lifestyle and reduces pain- and disability-associated depression (Morley & Sutherland, 2008).
Physical exercise and increasing fitness are common goals for pain management and can be useful to offset specific problems as a result of the pain or those associated with the primary condition. It is most useful if the majority of exercises used are those which the patients will be able to practice without the help of the therapist. Therapist contact time is limited and the establishment of a self-help programme is an essential aim of pain management (Watson, 2004).
Cognitive restructuring
A cognitive-behavioural model of pain and disability assumes that the way a person thinks about their condition can influence how they behave and feel. Common unhelpful thinking styles or cognitive distortions include catastrophizing (thinking the worst), overgeneralization (relating negative events to unconnected experiences), personalization (perceiving self is at fault) and selective abstraction (identifying negative experiences and ignoring positive ones). Once these are identified and the way in which they influence emotion and ultimately how this affects the way in which the person behaves towards their pain and disability, the therapist can help them identify the problems which stem from unhelpful thoughts, challenge these and identify different ways of helping them appraise situations and experiences (Thorn & Kahajda, 2006). Thought monitoring is an important component whereby the patient tries to distance themselves from their own behaviour and emotions and tries to rationalize it, identifying unhelpful thoughts and the emotional effect they have on them. In this way they can check themselves and try an alternative appraisal.
Maintenance
Chronic pain is characterized by periods of ‘flare-up’ with an accompanying restriction of physical activity, low mood and repeated health-care consultation. It is important that the lessons learned in the pain management programme are put into practice in the patient’s own environment. This is done with a combination of homework and practice and behavioural experimentation where the patient is asked to engage in specific behaviours they might find challenging – for example lifting objects with the affected limb. They then monitor and appraise the experience challenging their unhelpful beliefs. Eventually the patient is likely to become less fearful of the activity and increase activity. Specific paradigms to challenge fear of activity have been developed in musculoskeletal pain (Vlaeyen et al., 2002), but the evidence of their effectiveness in neurological conditions remains to be established.
Andersen G., Vestergaard K., Ingeman-Neilsen M., Jensen T. Incidence of central post-stroke pain. Pain. 1995;61:187-193.
Bennett M., Attal N., Backonja M., Baron R., Bouhassira D., Freynhagen R., et al. Using screening tools to identify neuropathic pain. Pain. 2007;127:199-203.
Bowsher D., Leijon G., Thuomas K.A. Central poststroke pain: correlation of MRI with clinical pain characteristics and sensory abnormalities. Neurology. 1998;51:1352-1358.
Cox S., Rice A. HIV and AIDS. In: Wilson P., Watson P.J., Haythornthwaite J.A., Jensen T., editors. Clinical Pain Management: Chronic Pain. 2nd edition. London: Hodder-Arnold; 2008:352-361.
Daly A.E., Bialocerkowski A.E. Does evidence support physiotherapy management of adult Complex Regional Pain Syndrome Type One? A systematic review. Eur. J. Pain. 2009;13(4):339-353.
Daniel H.C., Narewska J., Serpell M., Hoggart B., Johnson R., Rice A.S.C. Comparison of psychological and physical function in neuropathic pain and nociceptive pain: implications for cognitive behavioral pain management programs. Eur. J. Pain. 2008;12(6):731-741.
Dejerine J., Roussy G. La syndrome thalamique. Rev. Neurol. (Paris). 1906;14:521-532.
Dworkin R.H., O’Connor A.B., Backonja M., Farrar J., Finnerup N., Jensen T., et al. Pharmalogical management of neuropathic pain: Evidence based recommendations. Pain. 2007;132:237-251.
Eccleston C., Williams A.C.D.C., Morley S. Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.. (2):2009. CD007407
Eide P., Stubhaug A., Stenehjem A. Central dysesthesia pain after traumatic spinal cord injury is dependent on N-Methyl-D-aspartate receptor activation. Neurosurgery. 1995;37:1080-1087.
Herman R., D’Luzansky S., Ippolito R. Intrathecal Baclofen suppressed central pain in patients with spinal cord injury. Clin. J. Pain. 1992;8:338-345.
Loubser P., Akman N. Effects of intrathecal Baclofen on chronic spinal cord injury pain. J. Pain Symptom Manage.. 1996;12:241-247.
MacGowan D., Janal M., Clark W. Central post-stoke pain and Wallenberg’s lateral medullary infarction: frequency character and determinants in 63 patients. Neurology. 1997;49:120-125.
Main C., Sullivan M.J.L., Watson P.J. Pain Management: Practical applications of the biopsychosocial perspective in clinical and occupational settings. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, 2008.
Merskey H., Bogduk N. Classification of chronic pain: descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Seattle: IASP Press, 1994.
Morley S., Sutherland R. Self-pain enmeshment: future posible selves, sociotropy, autonomy and adjustment to chronic pain. Pain. 2008;137:366-377.
Morley S., Williams A.C.D.C., Hussain S. Estimating the clinical effectiveness of cognitive behavioural therapy in the clinic: evaluation of a CBT informed pain management programme. Pain. 2008;137:670-680.
Moseley G.L. Using visual illusion to reduce at-level neuropathic pain in paraplegia. Pain. 2007;130:294-298.
Moseley G.L., Gallace A., Spence C. Is mirror therapy all it is cracked up to be? Current evidence and future directions. Pain. 2008;138:7-10.
Moulin D., Foley K., Ebers G. Pain syndromes in Multiple Sclerosis. Neurology. 1988;38:1830-1834.
NICE. Neuropathic pain: the pharmacological management of neuropathic pain in adults in non-specialist settings. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2010.
Osterberg A., Boivie J., Thuomas K.A. Central pain in multiple sclerosis – prevalence and clinical characteristics. Eur. J. Pain. 2005;9:531-542.
Rog D., Nurmikko T., Freide T., Young C. Validation and reliability of the Neuropathic Pain Scale (NPS) in multiple sclerosis. Clin. J. Pain. 2007;23:473-481.
Siddall P., McClelland J., Rutkowski S., Cousins M. A longitudinal study of the prevalence and characteristics of pain in the first 5 years following spinal cord injury. Pain. 2003;103:249-257.
Siddall P., Yezierski R., Loeser J.D. Taxonomy and epidemiology of spinal cord injury pain. Yezierski R., editor. Spinal cord injury pain: assessment, mechanisms, management. Progress in pain research and management. Seattle: IASP Press. 2002.
Solaro C., Brichetto G., Amato M. The prevelance of pain in Multiple Sclerosis: A Multicentre cross-sectional study. Neurology. 2004;63:919-921.
Sullivan M.J.L. Toward a biopsychomotor conceptualization of pain: implications for research and intervention. Clin. J. Pain. 2008;24(4):281-290.
Thorn B., Kahajda M. Group cognitive therapy for chronic pain. J. Clin. Psychol.. 2006;62:1355-1366.
Treede R., Jensen T., Campbell J. Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes. Neurology. 2008;70:1630-1635.
Vlaeyen J.W.S., De Jong J., Geilen M., Heuts P., Van Breukelen G. The treatment of fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain: Further evidence on the effectiveness of exposure in vivo. Clin. J. Pain. 2008;18(4):251-261.
Wallace V., Rice A. Applied physiology: neuropathic pain. In: Wilson P., Watson P., Haythornthwaite J.A., Jensen T., editors. Clinical Pain Management: Chronic Pain. 2nd edition. London: Hodder-Arnold; 2008:3-23.
Watson P.J. Managing chronic pain. Boyling J., Jull G., editors. Grieve’s Modern Manual Therapy: the vertebral column. Edinburgh: Churchill-Livingston. 2004:551-566.