Intellectual Disability

Published on 22/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Pediatrics

Last modified 22/04/2025

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 2070 times

Chapter 33 Intellectual Disability

Intellectual disability (formerly called mental retardation) refers to a group of disorders that have in common deficits of adaptive and intellectual function and an age of onset before maturity is reached.

Definition

Three criteria must be met to establish the diagnosis of intellectual disability: significantly subaverage intellectual function, significant impairments in adaptive function, and onset before 18 years of age. The three diagnostic formulations—Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR), American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD), and Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA)—agree on the 3 criteria but define them differently.

Significantly subaverage general intellectual function refers to performance on an individually administered test of intelligence that is approximately two standard deviations (SD) below the mean. For a test that has a mean of 100 and SD of 15, IQ scores below 70 would meet these criteria. If the standard error of measurement is considered, the upper limits of subaverage intellectual function may extend to an IQ of 75. Using a score of 75 to delineate intellectual disability might double the number of children with intellectual disability, but the requirement for impairment of adaptive skills limits the false positives. Children with intellectual disability often show a variable pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Not all of their partial scores on IQ tests fall into the significantly subaverage range.

Significant impairment in adaptive behavior reflects the degree that the cognitive dysfunction impairs daily function. Adaptive behavior refers to the skills that are required for people to function in their everyday lives. Adaptive behavior may be assessed by three different constructs: the classification of DSM-IV-TR, the classification of AAIDD, and the IDEA.

The DSM-IV-TR classification of adaptive behavior addresses 10 domains: communication, self-care, home living, social and interpersonal skills, use of community resources, self-direction, functional academics, work, leisure, and health and safety. For a deficit in adaptive behavior to be present, a significant delay in 2 of the 10 areas must be present.

The AAIDD classification of adaptive behavior addresses 3 broad sets of skills: conceptual, social, and practical. Conceptual skills include language, reading and writing, money concepts, and self-direction. Social skills include interpersonal skills, personal responsibility, self-esteem, gullibility, naiveté, and ability to follow rules, obey laws, and avoid victimization. Representative practical skills are performance of activities of daily living (dressing, feeding, toileting and bathing, mobility), instrumental activities of daily living (housework, managing money, taking medication, shopping, preparing meals, using the telephone, etc) occupational skills, and the maintenance of a safe environment. For a deficit in adaptive behavior to be present, a significant delay in 1 of the 3 areas must be present. The rationale for requiring only 1 of the 3 areas is the empirically derived finding that people with intellectual disability can have varying patterns of ability and may not have deficits in all 3 areas.

The IDEA requires that the cognitive dysfunction affect school performance.

The requirement for adaptive behavior deficits is the most controversial aspect of the diagnostic formulation. The controversy centers on 2 broad areas: whether impairments in adaptive behavior are necessary for the construct of intellectual disability and what to measure. The adaptive behavior criterion may be irrelevant for many children; adaptive behavior is impaired in virtually all children who have IQ scores <50. The major utility of the adaptive behavior criterion is to confirm intellectual disability in children with IQ scores in the 65-75 range. It should be noted that deficits in adaptive behavior are often found in disorders such as Asperger syndrome (Chapter 28) and ADHD (Chapter 30) in the presence of typical intellectual function.

The issues of measurement are important as well. The independence of the 3 domains of the AAIDD and the 10 domains of the DSM-IV-TR has not been validated with research. The relationship between adaptive behavior and IQ performance is insufficiently explored. Most adults with mild intellectual disability do not have significant impairments in practical skills. It should be noted that adaptive behavior deficits must be distinguished from maladaptive behavior (e.g., aggression, inappropriate sexual contact).

Onset before age 18 yr distinguishes dysfunctions that originate during the developmental period. The diagnosis of intellectual disability may be made after 18 years of age, but the cognitive and adaptive dysfunction must have been manifested before age 18. The IDEA, because of its focus on school-aged children, does not require a limit of 18 years but refers to the “developmental period.”

The most commonly used medical diagnostic criteria for intellectual disability are those contained in the DSM-IV-TR (Table 33-1). The classification of intellectual disability that results from these definitions has been criticized for depending on IQ test performance rather than adaptive behavior, not taking the standard error of measurement into account, and not being predictive of outcomes for individuals. A new edition is currently being prepared that might address these issues. The AAIDD has proposed a different classification system. Instead of defining degrees of deficit (mild to profound), the AAIDD definition substitutes levels of support required in areas of adaptive function (intermittent, limited, extensive, or pervasive). The reliability of this approach has been challenged, and it blurs the distinction between intellectual and other developmental disabilities (communication disorder, autism, specific learning disabilities).

The term mental retardation should be cast aside because it is stigmatizing, has been used to limit the achievements of the individual, and has not met its initial objective of providing assistance to people with the disorder. The term intellectual disability is increasingly used in its place but has not been adopted universally; existing laws and their attendant entitlements still use the term mental retardation. In Europe, the term learning disability is often used to describe intellectual disability. Global developmental delay is a term often used to describe young children whose limitations have not yet resulted in a formal diagnosis of intellectual disability; it is often inappropriately used beyond the point when it is clear the child has intellectual disability, usually age 3 years.

Etiology

There appear to be 2 overlapping populations of children with intellectual disability: mild (IQ >50-70), which is more associated with environmental influences, and severe (IQ < 50), which is more frequently linked to biologic causes. Mild intellectual disability is 4 times more likely to be found in the offspring of women who have not completed high school than in women who have graduated. This is presumably a consequence of both genetic (children can inherit an intellectual impairment) and socioeconomic (poverty, malnutrition) factors. The specific causes of mild intellectual disability are identifiable in <50% of affected individuals. The most common biologic causes of mild intellectual disability include genetic or chromosomal syndromes with multiple, major, or minor congenital anomalies (velocardiofacial syndrome, Williams syndrome, Noonan’s syndrome), intrauterine growth restriction, prematurity, perinatal insults, intrauterine exposure to drugs of abuse (including alcohol), and sex chromosomal abnormalities. Familial clustering is common.

In children with severe intellectual disability, a biologic cause (most commonly prenatal) can be identified in >75% of cases. Causes include chromosomal (e.g., Down syndrome Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome, deletion 1p36 syndrome) and other genetic and epigenetic disorders (e.g., fragile X syndrome, Rett syndrome, Angelman and Prader-Willi syndromes), abnormalities of brain development (e.g., lissencephaly), and inborn errors of metabolism or neurodegenerative disorders (e.g, mucopolysaccharidoses) (Table 33-2). Consistent with the finding that disorders that alter early embryogenesis are the most common and severe, the earlier the problem occurs in development, the more severe its consequences tend to be.

Table 33-2 IDENTIFICATION OF CAUSE IN CHILDREN WITH SEVERE INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

CAUSE EXAMPLES PERCENT OF TOTAL
Chromosomal disorder

∼20 Genetic syndrome ∼20 Nonsyndromic autosomal mutations Variations in copy number, Mutations in SYNGAP1, GRIK2, TUSC3 and oligosaccharyl transferase ∼10 Developmental brain abnormality Hydrocephalus ± meningomyelocele, lissencephaly ∼8 Inborn errors of metabolism or neurodegenerative disorder PKU, Tay-Sachs, various storage diseases ∼7 Congenital infections HIV, toxoplasmosis, rubella, CMV, syphilis, herpes simplex ∼3 Familial intellectual disability Environment, syndromic, or genetic ∼5 Perinatal causes HIE, meningitis, IVH, PVL, fetal alcohol syndrome 4 Postnatal causes Trauma (abuse), meningitis, hypothyroidism ∼4 Unknown Cerebral palsy 20

CMV, cytomegalovirus; HIE, hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy; HIV, human immunodeficiency virus; IVH, intraventricular hemorrhage; PKU, phenylketonuria; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia.

Modified from Stromme P, Hayberg G: Aetiology in severe and mild mental retardation: a population based study of Norwegian children, Dev Med Child Neurol 42:76–86, 2000.

Epidemiology

The prevalence of intellectual disability depends on the definition, the method of ascertainment, and the population. According to statistics (based on the DSM-IV-TR definition), 2.5% of the population should have intellectual disability, and 85% of these individuals should fall into the mild range. In 2005-2006, ∼556,000 or only 1.1% of school-aged children received services for intellectual disability in federally supported school programs in the US. For several reasons, fewer children than predicted are identified as having mild intellectual disability. Because it is more difficult to diagnose mild intellectual disability than the more severe forms, professionals might defer the diagnosis and give the benefit of the doubt to the child. Other reasons that contribute to the discrepancy are use of instruments that underidentify young children with mild intellectual disability, some children being diagnosed as having autism spectrum disorders and their intellectual disability not addressed, and a disinclination to make the diagnosis in poor or minority students because of previous overdiagnosis.

Young children might show cognitive limitations without significant delays in adaptive behavior. As a result, new cases of mild intellectual disability continue to be diagnosed among children up to 9 yr of age. Children with intellectual disability also may be incorporated into another diagnosis (e.g., autism, cerebral palsy). Furthermore, it is possible that the number of children with mild intellectual disability is actually decreasing as a result of public health and education measures to prevent prematurity and provide early intervention and head start programs. In fact, the number of school children who receive services for intellectual disability has not changed appreciably since 1997.

Unlike mild intellectual disability, where the prevalence may be decreasing, the occurrence of severe intellectual disability has not changed appreciably since the 1940s and is 0.3-0.5% of the population. Many of the causes of severe intellectual disability involve genetic or congenital brain malformations that can neither be anticipated nor treated at present. In addition, new populations with severe intellectual disability have offset the decreases in the prevalence of severe intellectual disability that have resulted from improved health care. Although prenatal diagnosis and subsequent pregnancy terminations have resulted in a decreased prevalence of Down syndrome (Chapter 76), and newborn screening with early treatment has virtually eliminated intellectual disability caused by phenylketonuria and congenital hypothyroidism, an increased prevalence of maternal prenatal drug use (Chapter 90.4) and improved survival of very low birthweight premature infants has counterbalanced this effect.

Overall, intellectual disability occurs more in boys than in girls: 2 : 1 in mild intellectual disability and 1.5 : 1 in severe intellectual disability. In part this may be a consequence of the many X-linked disorders associated with intellectual disability, the most prominent being fragile X syndrome.

Pathology and Pathogenesis

The limitations in our knowledge of the neuropathology of intellectual disability are exemplified by the fact that 10-20% of brains of persons with severe intellectual disability appear entirely normal by standard neuropathologic study. The majority of brains of these persons show only mild, nonspecific changes that correlate poorly with the degree of intellectual disability. These changes include microcephaly, gray matter heterotopias in the subcortical white matter, unusually regular columnar arrangement of the cortex, and neurons that are more tightly packed than usual. Only a minority of the brain shows more specific changes in dendritic and synaptic organization, with dysgenesis of dendritic spines or cortical pyramidal neurons, or impaired growth of dendritic trees.

The programming of the central nervous system (CNS) involves a process of induction; CNS maturation is defined in terms of genetic, molecular, autocrine, paracrine, and endocrine influences. Receptors, signaling molecules, and genes are critical to brain development. The maintenance of different neuronal phenotypes in the adult brain involves the same genetic transcripts that play a crucial role during fetal development, with activation of similar intracellular signal transduction mechanisms. Several syndromes that were thought to involve complex chromosomal abnormalities are, in fact, caused by single gene mutations involving induction. Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome (Chapter 76), a disorder marked clinically by broad thumbs and great toes, characteristic facies, and severe intellectual disability, has been shown to result from a mutation in the gene encoding for the transcriptional co-activator CREB binding protein (CBP), a factor important in the control of gene expression in early embryogenesis.

Clinical Manifestations

Early diagnosis of intellectual disability facilitates earlier intervention, identification of abilities, realistic goal setting, easing of parental anxiety, and greater acceptance of the child in the community. Most children with intellectual disability first come to the pediatrician’s attention in infancy because of dysmorphisms, associated developmental disabilities, or failure to meet age-appropriate developmental milestones. There are no specific physical characteristics of intellectual disability, but dysmorphisms may be the earliest signs that bring children to the attention of the pediatrician. They might fall within a genetic syndrome such as Down syndrome or be isolated, as in microcephaly or failure to thrive. Associated developmental disabilities include seizure disorders, cerebral palsy, hypotonia, and autism; these conditions are seen more commonly in conjunction with intellectual disability than in the general population.

Most children with intellectual disability do not keep up with their peers and fail to meet age-expected norms. In early infancy, failure to meet age-appropriate expectations can include a lack of visual or auditory responsiveness, unusual muscle tone (hypo- or hypertonia) or posture, and feeding difficulties. Between 6 and 18 mo of age, gross motor delay (lack of sitting, crawling, walking) is the most common complaint. Language delay and behavior problems are common concerns after 18 mo (Table 33-3). Earlier identification of atypical development is likely to occur with more severe impairments; and intellectual disability is usually identifiable by age 3 yr.

Table 33-3 COMMON PRESENTATIONS OF INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY BY AGE

AGE AREA OF CONCERN
Newborn

Early infancy (2-4 mo) Later infancy (6-18 mo) Gross motor delay Toddlers (2-3 yr) Language delays or difficulties Preschool (3-5 yr) School age (>5 yr)

For some children with mild intellectual disability the diagnosis remains uncertain during the early school years. It is only after the demands of the school setting increase over the years, changing from “learning to read” to “reading to learn,” that the child’s limitations are clarified.

Adolescents with mild intellectual disability can present a diagnostic challenge. Adolescents with mild intellectual disability “talk the talk” but do not “walk the walk.” Typically they are up to date on current trends and are conversant as to who, what, and where. It isn’t until the “why” and “how” questions are asked that their limitations become apparent. If allowed to interact at a superficial level, their mild intellectual disability might not be appreciated, even by professionals who may be their special education teachers or health care providers. Because of the stigma associated with intellectual disability, they may use euphemisms to avoid being thought of as “stupid” or “retarded” and refer to themselves as learning disabled, dyslexic, language disordered, or slow learners. Some people with intellectual disability emulate their social milieu to be accepted. They may be social chameleons and assume the morals of the group to which they are attached. Some would rather be thought “bad” than “incompetent.”

Laboratory Findings

The most commonly used medical diagnostic testing for children with intellectual disability include neuroimaging; metabolic, genetic, and chromosomal testing; microarray analysis; and electroencephalography (EEG). These tests should not be used as screening tools for all children with an intellectual disability. In some children, there is a reasonable yield for testing, whereas in others the yield of <1% does not support its use. Decisions on diagnostic testing should be based on the medical and family history, physical examination, testing by other disciplines, and the family’s wishes (Fig. 33-1). Table 33-4 summarizes clinical practice guidelines that have been published to assist in evaluating the child with global developmental delay or intellectual disability. Karyotyping, particularly focusing on the number of chromosomes, duplications, deletions, or chromosomal translocations and the subtelomeric region (a hot spot), is indicated in children with multiple anomalies or a positive family history. Microarray analysis for copy number variation detects deletions and duplications when traditional chromosome banding techniques are normal and should be performed if a normal karyotype is reported and other tests are unrevealing. Deletion 1p36 syndrome, the most common subtelomeric microdeletion syndrome (1 : 5,000 births), accounts for ∼1% of children with developmental disabilities and is characterized by failure to thrive, microcephaly, deep-set eyes, midface hypoplasia, broad nasal bridge, heart deficits, and CNS anomalies. Noncompaction cardiomyopathy and seizures are also noted. The diagnosis is made by standard chromosomes in only ∼20% and requires fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) or microarray comparative genomic hybridization methods for remaining patients.

Table 33-4 SUGGESTED EVALUATION OF THE CHILD WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY/GLOBAL DEVELOPMENTAL DELAY

TEST COMMENT
In-depth history Includes pre-, peri- and postnatal events (including seizures); developmental attainments; and 3-generation pedigree in family history
Physical examination

Vision and hearing evaluation Essential to detect and treat; can mask as developmental delay Karyotype Must include microarray copy number variation determinates if karyotype and chromosome banding are normal Fragile X screen Preselection on clinical grounds can increase yield to 7.6% Neuroimaging Thyroid (T4, TSH) Near 0% in settings with universal newborn screening program Serum lead If there are identifiable risk factors for excessive environmental lead exposure Metabolic testing Urine organic acids, plasma amino acids, ammonia, lactate, and a capillary blood gas. Focused testing based on clinical findings is warranted. Subtelomeric deletion replaced by CGH Obtain in the presence of dysmorphisms but with a normal karyotype and fragile X DNA study. Higher in severe intellectual disability. MECP2 for Rett syndrome Females with severe intellectual disability EEG May be deferred in absence of history of seizures Repeated history and physical examination Can give time for maturation of physical and behavioral phenotype. New technology may be available for evaluation.

CGH, comparative genomic hybridization; EEG, electroencephalogram; T4, thyroxine; TSH, thyroid-stimulating hormone.

Based on Curry et al, 1997; Shapiro BK, Batshaw ML: Mental retardation. In Burg FD, Ingelfinger JR, Polin RA, et al: Gellis and Kagan’s current pediatric therapy, ed 18, Philadelphia, 2005, WB Saunders, used with permission; and Shevell M, Ashwal S, Donley D, et al: Practice parameter: evaluation of the child with global developmental delay, Neurology 60:367–380, 2003.

Molecular genetic testing for fragile X syndrome is appropriate for a boy with moderate intellectual disability, unusual physical features, and/or a family history of intellectual disability or for a girl with more subtle cognitive deficits associated with severe shyness and a relevant family history. A child with a progressive neurologic disorder or acute behavioral changes needs metabolic investigation (urinary organic acids, plasma amino acids, blood lactate, lysosomal enzymes in lymphocytes); a child with seizure-like episodes should have an EEG performed. Children with micro- or macrocephaly or changes in head growth trajectory or asymmetric head shapes, as well as those with new or focal neurologic findings, including seizures, should have a neuroimaging procedure.

Some children with more subtle physical or neurologic findings can also have determinable biologic causes of their intellectual disability. About 6% of unexplained intellectual disability can be accounted for by micro chromosomal abnormalities that can be identified by high-resolution chromosomal banding, FISH, or chromosome painting for subtelomeric rearrangements. Microarray genome analysis with gene chip technology is replacing subtelomeric FISH probes; it can identify variants of unknown significance or benign variants, and therefore it should be used in conjunction with a genetic consultation. Magnetic resonance imaging scans identify a significant number of subtle markers of cerebral dysgenesis in children with intellectual disability. Formes frustes of amino acid and organic acid disorders are associated with intellectual disability in the absence of the more commonly associated manifestations of behavior change, lethargy, and coma.

How intensively one investigates the cause of a child’s intellectual disability is based on a number of factors:

Differential Diagnosis

One of the important roles of pediatricians is the early recognition and diagnosis of cognitive deficits. The developmental surveillance approach to early diagnosis of intellectual disability should be multifaceted. Parents’ concerns and observations about their child’s development should be listened to carefully, because their observations have been found to be as accurate as developmental screening tests. Medical, genetic, and environmental risk factors should be recognized. Infants at high risk (prematurity, maternal substance abuse, perinatal insult) should be registered in newborn follow-up programs in which they are evaluated periodically for developmental lags in the first 2 yr of life; they should be referred to early intervention (Child Find) programs as appropriate. Developmental milestones should be recorded routinely during health care maintenance visits. The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) has formulated a schema for developmental surveillance and screening. Whether developmental surveillance is a more effective technique for identifying than recognizing failure to meet age-appropriate milestones has not been clearly established.

Before making the diagnosis of intellectual disability, other disorders that affect cognitive abilities and adaptive behavior should be considered. These include conditions that mimic intellectual disability and others that involve intellectual disability as an associated impairment. Sensory deficits (severe hearing and vision loss), communication disorders, and poorly controlled seizure disorders can mimic intellectual disability; certain progressive neurologic disorders can appear as intellectual disability before regression is appreciated. More than half of children with cerebral palsy (Chapter 591.1) or autism spectrum disorders (Chapter 28) also have intellectual disability as an associated deficit. Differentiation of isolated cerebral palsy from intellectual disability relies on motor skills being more affected than cognitive skills and on the presence of pathologic reflexes and tone changes. In autism spectrum disorders, language and social adaptive skills are more affected than nonverbal reasoning skills, whereas in intellectual disability there are usually more equivalent deficits in social, motor, adaptive, and cognitive skills.

Diagnostic Psychologic Testing

The formal diagnosis of intellectual disability requires the administration of individual tests of intelligence and adaptive functioning.

The Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II), the most commonly used infant intelligence scale, assesses language, visual problem-solving skills, behavior, fine motor skills, and gross motor skills in children between 1 mo and 3 yr of age. A Mental Developmental Index (MDI) and a Psychomotor Development Index score (PDI, a measure of motor competence) are derived from the results. This test permits the differentiation of infants with severe intellectual disability from typically developing infants, but it is less helpful in distinguishing between a typical child and one with mild intellectual disability.

The most commonly used psychologic tests for children >3 yr of age are the Wechsler Scales. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 3rd edition (WPPSI-III) is used for children with mental ages of 2.5-7.3 yr. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edition (WISC-IV), is used for children who function above a 6 yr mental age. Both scales contain a number of subtests in the areas of verbal and performance skills. Although children with intellectual disability usually score below average on all subscale scores, they occasionally score in the average range in one or more performance areas.

The most commonly used test of adaptive behavior is the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS), which involves semistructured interviews with parents and/or caregivers and teachers that assess adaptive behavior in four domains: communication, daily living skills, socialization, and motor skills. Other tests of adaptive behavior include the Woodcock-Johnson Scales of Independent Behavior—Revised, the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disability Adaptive Behavior Scale (ABS-2nd edition) and the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (ABAS-2nd edition). There is usually (but not always) a good correlation between scores on the intelligence and adaptive scales. Basic adaptive abilities (feeding, dressing, hygiene) are more responsive to remedial efforts than is the IQ score. Adaptive abilities are also more variable, which can relate to the underlying condition and to environmental expectations. Although persons with Prader-Willi syndrome (Chapter 76) have stability of adaptive skills through adulthood, those with fragile X syndrome (Chapter 76) may have increasing deficits over time.

Treatment

Although intellectual disability is not treatable, many associated impairments are amenable to intervention and therefore benefit from early identification. Most children with an intellectual disability do not have a behavioral or emotional disorder as an associated impairment, but challenging behaviors (aggression, self-injury, oppositional defiant behavior) and mental illness (mood and anxiety disorders) occur with greater frequency in this population than among children with typical intelligence. These behavioral and emotional disorders are the primary cause for out-of-home placements, reduced employment prospects, and decreased opportunities for social integration. Some behavioral and emotional disorders are difficult to diagnose in children with more severe intellectual disability because of the child’s limited abilities to understand, communicate, interpret, or generalize. Other disorders are masked by the intellectual disability. The detection of ADHD (Chapter 30) in the presence of moderate to severe intellectual disability may be difficult, as may be discerning a thought disorder (psychosis) in someone with autism and intellectual disability.

Although mental illness is generally of biologic origin and responds to medication, behavioral disorders can result from a mismatch between the child’s abilities and the demands of the situation, organic problems, and/or family difficulties. They may represent attempts by the child to communicate, gain attention, or avoid frustration. In assessing the challenging behavior, one must also consider whether it is inappropriate for the child’s mental age, rather than the chronological age. When intervention is needed, an environmental change, such as a more appropriate classroom setting, may improve certain behavior problems. Behavior management techniques are useful; psychopharmacologic agents may be appropriate in certain situations.

Medication is not useful in treating the core symptoms of intellectual disability; no agent has been found to improve intellectual function. Medication may be helpful in treating associated behavioral and psychiatric disorders. Psychopharmacology is generally directed at specific symptom complexes including ADHD (stimulant medication), self-injurious behavior and aggression (neuroleptics), and anxiety obsessive-compulsive disorder, and depression (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors). Before long-term therapy with any psychopharmacologic agent is initiated, a short trial should be conducted. Even if a medication proves successful, its use should be re-evaluated at least yearly to assess the need for continued treatment.

Supportive Care and Management

Each child with intellectual disability needs a medical home with a pediatrician who is readily accessible to the family to answer questions, help coordinate care, and discuss concerns. Pediatricians can have effects on patients and their families that are still felt decades later. The role of the pediatrician includes involvement in prevention efforts, early diagnosis, identification of associated deficits, referral for appropriate diagnostic and therapeutic services, interdisciplinary management, provision of primary care, and advocacy for the child and family. The management strategies for children with an intellectual disability should be multimodal, with efforts directed at all aspects of the child’s life: health, education, social and recreational activities, behavior problems, and associated impairments. Support for parents and siblings should also be provided.

Prognosis

In children with severe intellectual disability, the prognosis is often evident by early childhood. Mild intellectual disability might not always be a lifelong disorder. Children might meet criteria for intellectual disability at an early age, but later the disability can evolve into a more specific developmental disorder (communication disorder, autism, slow learner, or borderline normal intelligence). Others with a diagnosis of mild intellectual disability during their school years develop sufficient adaptive behavior skills so that they no longer fit the diagnosis as adolescents, or the effects of maturation and plasticity can result in children moving from one diagnostic category to another (from moderate to mild retardation). Some children who have a diagnosis of a specific learning disability or communication disorder might not maintain their rate of cognitive growth and fall into the range of intellectual disability over time. By adolescence, the diagnosis has generally stabilized.

The long-term outcome of persons with intellectual disability depends on the underlying cause, the degree of cognitive and adaptive deficits, the presence of associated medical and developmental impairments, the capabilities of the families, and the school and community supports, services, and training provided to the child and family (Table 33-5). As adults, many persons with mild intellectual disability are capable of gaining economic and social independence with functional literacy. They might need periodic supervision, especially when under social or economic stress. Most live successfully in the community, either independently or in supervised settings. Life expectancy is not adversely affected by intellectual disability itself.

Table 33-5 SEVERITY OF INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY AND ADULT AGE FUNCTIONING

LEVEL MENTAL AGE AS ADULT* ADULT ADAPTATION
Mild 9-11 yr Reads at 4th-5th grade level; simple multiplication and division; writes simple letter, lists; completes job application; basic independent job skills (arrive on time, stay at task, interact with coworkers); uses public transportation, might qualify for driver’s license; keeps house, cooks using recipes
Moderate 6-8 yr Sight-word reading; copies information, e.g., address from card to job application; matches written number to number of items; recognizes time on clock; communicates; some independence in self-care; housekeeping with supervision or cue cards; meal preparation, can follow picture recipe cards; job skills learned with much repetition; uses public transportation with some supervision
Severe 3-5 yr Needs continuous support and supervision; might communicate wants and needs, sometimes with augmentative communication techniques
Profound <3 yr Limitations of self-care, continence, communication, and mobility; might need complete custodial or nursing care

* International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th edition (World Health Organization).

From Dr. Robert L. Schum, Grand Rounds Presentation at Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin, 2003.

For persons with moderate intellectual disability, the goals of education are to enhance adaptive abilities and “survival” academic and vocational skills so they are better able to live in the adult world (see Table 33-5). The concept of supported employment has been very beneficial to these individuals; the person is trained by a coach to do a specific job in the setting in which the person is to work. This bypasses the need for a sheltered workshop experience and has resulted in successful work adaptation in the community for many people with an intellectual disability. These persons generally live at home or in a supervised setting in the community.

As adults, people with severe to profound intellectual disability usually require extensive to pervasive supports (see Table 33-5). These individuals may have associated impairments, such as cerebral palsy, behavioral disorders, epilepsy, or sensory impairments, that further limit their adaptive functioning. They can perform simple tasks in supervised settings. Most people with this level of intellectual disability are able to live in the community with appropriate supports.

Bibliography

Allen-Leigh B, Katz G, Rangel-Eudave G, et al. View of Mexican family members on the autonomy of adolescents and adults with intellectual disability. Salud Publica Mex. 2008;50(suppl 2):s213-s221.

American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Children with Disabilities. Pediatrician’s role in the development and implementation of an Individualized Education Plan (IEP) and/or an Individual Family Service Plan (IFSP). Pediatrics. 1999;104:124-127.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

Batshaw ML, Shapiro BK, Farber MLZ. Developmental delay and intellectual disability. In: Batshaw ML, Pellegrino L, Roizen NJ, editors. Children with disabilities. ed 6. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes; 2007:245-262.

Battaglia A, Hoyme HE, Dallapiccola B, et al. Further delineation of deletion 1p36 syndrome in 60 patients: a recognizable phenotype and common cause of developmental delay and mental retardation. Pediatrics. 2008;121:404-410.

Council on Children with Disabilities, Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Steering Committee, Medical Home Initiatives for Children with Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics. 2006;118:405-420.

Hamdan FF, Gauthier J, Spiegelman D, et al. Mutations in SYNGAP1 in autosomal nonsyndromic mental retardation. N Engl J Med. 2009;360:599-605.

Johnson CP, Walker WOJr, Palomo-Gonzales SA, et al. Mental retardation: diagnosis, management, and family support. Curr Prob Pediatr Adolesc Health Care. 2006;36:123-171.

Moeschler JB. Genetic evaluation of intellectual disabilities. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2008;15(1):2-9.

Rao LG. Education of persons with intellectual disabilities in India. Salud PublicaMe. 2008;50(Suppl 2)):s205-s212.

Roeleveld N, Zielhuis GA, Gabreëls F. The prevalence of mental retardation: a critical review of recent literature. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1997;39:125-132.

Saha S, Barnett AG, Foldi C, et al. Advanced paternal age is associated with impaired neurocognitive outcomes during infancy and childhood. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e40.

Shapiro BK, Batshaw ML. Mental retardation. In: Burg FD, Ingelfinger JR, Polin RA, et al, editors. Current pediatric therapy. ed 18. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 2006:1250-1252.

Shevell M, Ashwal S, Donley D, et al. Practice parameter: evaluation of the child with global developmental delay. Neurology. 2003;60:367-380.

Strømme P, Hagberg G. Aetiology in severe and mild mental retardation: a population-based study of Norwegian children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42:76-86.

Wilson-Costello D, Friedman H, Minich N, et al. Improved survival rates with increased neurodevelopmental disability for extremely low birth weight infants in the 1990s. Pediatrics. 2005;115:997-1003.

Yeargin-Allsopp M, Boyle C. The epidemiology of neurodevelopmental disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2002;8:113-211.