Dementia

Published on 03/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Neurology

Last modified 03/03/2015

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 0 (0 votes)

This article have been viewed 3943 times

Chapter 7 Dementia

Neurologists and most other physicians continue to use the term dementia, which they see as a clinical condition or syndrome of a progressive decline in cognitive function that impairs daily activities. Neurologists require memory impairment plus one or more of the following: aphasia, apraxia, agnosia, or disturbance in executive function (see Chapter 8). Because their definition requires two domains, it excludes isolated amnesia (Greek, forgetfulness) or aphasia (Greek, speechlessness).

Psychiatrists adopting at least the preliminary version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, 5th edition (DSM-5) will use the term Neurocognitive Disorder and its subtypes, Mild and Major. Without specifying an underlying illness, psychiatrists may allow each subtype to stand on its own. Once they know the underlying diagnosis, they may associate Neurocognitive Disorder with a specific illness, e.g., Neurocognitive Disorders due to Alzheimer’s Disease. The criteria for both Mild and Major Neurocognitive Disorders require impairments that represent a decline from a previous level of performance. Mild Neurocognitive Disorder must not interfere with independence, but Major Neurocognitive Disorder is sufficiently severe to interfere with independence. Neither occurs exclusively in the context of delirium nor is attributable to another mental disorder, such as Major Depressive Disorder.

Disorders Related to Dementia

Amnesia

Memory loss with otherwise preserved intellectual function constitutes amnesia. Individuals with amnesia typically cannot recall recently presented information (retrograde amnesia), newly presented information (anterograde amnesia), or both (global amnesia). Although amnesia may occur as an isolated deficit, it appears more often as one of two or more components of dementia. In fact, amnesia is a requirement for the diagnosis of dementia.

Neurologists usually attribute amnesia to transient or permanent dysfunction of the hippocampus (Greek, sea horse) and other portions of the limbic system, which are based in the temporal and frontal lobes (see Fig. 16-5). Dementia, in contrast, usually results from extensive cerebral cortex dysfunction (see later).

Transient amnesia is an important, relatively common disturbance that usually consists of a suddenly occurring period of amnesia lasting only several minutes to several hours. It has several potential medical and neurologic explanations (Box 7-1). One of them, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), routinely induces both anterograde and retrograde amnesia, with retrograde amnesia, especially for autobiographic information, tending to persist longer than anterograde amnesia. ECT-induced amnesia is more likely to occur or to be more pronounced following treatment with high electrical dosage, with bilateral rather than unilateral electrode placement, with use of alternating current, and three-times rather than two-times weekly administration. Without a pretreatment assessment of a patient’s memory and other aspects of cognitive function, clinicians may have problems separating ECT-induced amnesia from memory difficulties reflecting underlying depression, medications, and, especially in the elderly, pre-existing cognitive impairment.

Various neuropsychologic and physical abnormalities usually accompany amnesia from neurologic conditions. For example, behavioral disturbances, depression, and headache are comorbid with posttraumatic amnesia. With severe traumatic brain injury (TBI, see Chapter 22), hemiparesis, ataxia, pseudobulbar palsy, or epilepsy accompanies posttraumatic amnesia. Similarly, in addition to its characteristic anterograde amnesia, the Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome comprises ataxia and signs of a peripheral neuropathy (see later).

In another example, herpes simplex encephalitis causes amnesia accompanied by personality changes, complex partial seizures, and the Klüver–Bucy syndrome (see Chapters 12 and 16) because the virus typically enters the undersurface of the brain through the nasopharynx and attacks the frontal and temporal lobes. This condition occurs relatively frequently because herpes simplex is the most common cause of sporadically occurring (nonepidemic) viral encephalitis. (Human immunodeficiency virus [HIV] encephalitis, which does not cause this scenario, is epidemic.)

Conversely, apparent memory impairment may also appear as an aspect of several psychiatric disorders (see Chapter 11, dissociative amnesia). In general, individuals with amnesia from psychiatric illness or malingering (nonneurologic amnesia) lose memory for personal identity or emotionally laden events rather than recently acquired information. For example, a criminal deeply in debt may travel to another city and “forget” his debts, wife, and past associates, but he would retain his ability to recall people, events, and day-to-day transactions in his new life. Nonneurologic amnesia also characteristically produces inconsistent results on formal memory testing. In a simple example, a workman giving emphasis to the sequelae of a head injury may seem unable to recall three playing cards after 30 seconds, but half an hour after discussing neutral topics will recall three different cards after a 5-minute interval. Also, Amytal infusions may temporarily restore memories in individuals with nonneurologic amnesia, but not in those with brain damage.

Neuropsychologic Conditions

Confabulation is a neuropsychologic condition in which patients offer implausible explanations in a sincere, forthcoming, and typically jovial manner. Although individuals with confabulation disregard the truth, they do not intentionally deceive. Confabulation is a well-known aspect of Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome, Anton syndrome (see cortical blindness, Chapter 12), and anosognosia (see Chapter 8). With these conditions referable to entirely different regions of the brain, confabulation lacks consistent anatomic correlations and physical features.

Discrete neuropsychologic disorders – aphasia, anosognosia, and apraxia – may occur alone, in various combinations, or as comorbidities of dementia (see Chapter 8). If one of them occurs with a comorbidity of dementia, it indicates that the dementia originates in “cortical” rather than “subcortical” dysfunction (see later). These disorders, unlike dementia, are attributable to discrete cerebral lesions. Sometimes only neuropsychologic testing can detect these disorders and tease them apart from dementia.

Normal Aging

Beginning at about age 50 years, people are subject to a variety of natural, age-related changes. Many neurologic functions resist age-related changes, but some are especially vulnerable. Those that decline do so at different rates and in uneven trajectories.

Dementia

Classifications and Causes

The traditional classification of “dementia by etiology” overwhelms clinicians because of the seemingly innumerable illnesses. Instead, each of the following classifications based on salient clinical features, although overlapping, provides practical and easy-to-learn approaches:

• Prevalence: Studies reporting on the epidemiology of dementia-producing illnesses vary by whether cases were clinical or postmortem, drawn from primary or tertiary care settings, or if the criteria were incidence or prevalence. By any measure, Alzheimer disease is the most prevalent cause of dementia, not only because its incidence is so high, but also, with its victims living for a relatively long time, its prevalence is also high. A typical breakdown of the prevalence of dementia-producing illnesses would list Alzheimer disease 70%, dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) 15%, vascular cognitive impairment (VCI) or the preliminary DSM-5 term Vascular Neurocognitive Impairment 10%, frontotemporal dementia 5–10%, and all others, in total, 5–10%.

• Patient’s age at the onset of dementia: Similarly, beginning at age 65 years, those same illnesses cause almost all cases of dementia. However, individuals between 21 and 65 years are liable to succumb to different dementia-producing illnesses: HIV disease, substance abuse, severe TBI, end-stage multiple sclerosis (MS) (see Chapter 15), frontotemporal dementia, and VCI. In adolescence, the causes are different and more numerous (Box 7-2).

• Accompanying physical manifestations: Distinctive physical neurologic abnormalities that may accompany dementia and allow for a diagnosis by inspection include ocular motility impairments, gait apraxia (see later), myoclonus (see later and Chapter 18), peripheral neuropathy (see Box 5-1), chorea, other involuntary movement disorders (see Box 18-4), and lateralized signs, such as hemiparesis.

• Genetics: Of frequently occurring illnesses, Huntington disease, frontotemporal dementia, some prion illnesses, and, in certain families, Alzheimer disease follow an autosomal dominant pattern. Wilson disease follows an autosomal recessive pattern.

• Rapidity of onset: In patients with Alzheimer disease, dementia evolves over a period of many years to a decade, which serves as the reference point. In contrast, several diseases produce dementia within 6–12 months. These “rapidly progressive dementias” include HIV-associated dementia, frontotemporal dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies (see later), paraneoplastic limbic encephalitis (see Chapter 19), and, perhaps most notoriously, Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease and its variant.

• Reversibility: The most common conditions that neurologists usually list as “reversible causes of dementia” are depression, over-medication, hypothyroidism, B12 deficiency, other metabolic abnormalities, subdural hematomas, and normal-pressure hydrocephalus (NPH). Although reversible dementias are rightfully sought, the results of treatment are discouraging. Only about 9% of dementia cases are potentially reversible and physicians actually partially or fully reverse less than 1%.

• Cortical and subcortical dementias: According to a cortical/subcortical distinction, cortical dementias consist of illnesses in which neuropsychologic signs of cortical injury – typically aphasia, agnosia, and apraxia – accompany dementia. Because the brain’s subcortical areas are relatively untouched, patients remain alert, attentive, and ambulatory. Alzheimer disease serves as the prime example of a cortical dementia.

Although the cortical/subcortical distinction persists, it is slipping into disuse because of several problems. The presence or absence of aphasia, agnosia, and apraxia does not reliably predict the category of dementia. In addition, this classification cannot account for the prominent exceptions inherent in several illnesses, including subcortical pathology in Alzheimer disease, the mixed clinical picture in frontotemporal dementia, and cortical pathology in Huntington disease.

Of the numerous dementia-producing illnesses, this chapter discusses Alzheimer disease, other frequently occurring dementia-producing illnesses, and several that are otherwise important. Under separate headings, this chapter discusses the companion topics of pseudodementia and delirium. Subsequent chapters discuss dementia-producing illnesses characterized by their physical manifestations.

Mental Status Testing

Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Fig. 7-1)

Physicians so regularly administer the MMSE that it has risen to the level of the standard screening test. Its results are reproducible and correlate with histologic changes in Alzheimer disease. In addition to detecting cognitive impairment, the MMSE has predictive value. For example, among well-educated individuals with borderline scores, as many as 10–25% may develop dementia in the next 2 years. The MMSE can also cast doubt on a diagnosis of Alzheimer disease as the cause of dementia under certain circumstances: (1) If scores on successive tests remain stable for 2 years, the diagnosis should be reconsidered because Alzheimer disease almost always causes a progressive decline. (2) If scores decline precipitously, illnesses that cause a rapidly progressive dementia become more likely diagnoses (see later).

Critics attack the MMSE. It is “too easy.” It permits mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and even early dementia to escape detection. Age, education, and language skills influence the score. The MMSE may also fail to test adequately for executive function and thereby miss cases of frontotemporal dementia. Also, because the MMSE depends so heavily on language function, it may be inadequate in measuring conditions, such as MS and toluene abuse, where the subcortical white matter receives the brunt of the damage.

Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS)

The ADAS consists of cognitive and noncognitive sections. Its cognitive section (ADAS-Cog) (Fig. 7-2) includes not only standard tests of language, comprehension, memory, and orientation, but also tests of visuospatial ability, such as drawing geometric figures, and physical tasks that reflect ideational praxis, such as folding a paper into an envelope. Patients obtain scores of 0–70 points in proportion to worsening performance.

Compared to the MMSE, the ADAS-Cog is more sensitive, reliable, and less influenced by educational level and language skills. However, it is more complex and subjective. Test-givers, who need not be physicians, must undergo special training. The testing usually requires 45–60 minutes. Alzheimer disease researchers, especially those involved in pharmaceutical trials, routinely use the ADAS-Cog to monitor the course of Alzheimer disease and measure the effect of medication.

Laboratory Evaluation in Dementia

Depending on the clinical evaluation, neurologists generally request a series of laboratory tests (Box 7-3). Although testing is expensive (see Appendix 2), it may allow a firm diagnosis or even detect a potentially correctable cause of dementia. In addition, certain tests may reveal the illness before individuals manifest cognitive impairment, i.e., in their preclinical or presymptomatic state. Neurologists modify the testing protocol if dementia has developed in an adolescent (see Box 7-2) or has progressed rapidly.

Because CT can detect most structural abnormalities associated with dementia, it is a sufficient screening test. However, MRI is superior because it is better able to diagnose multiple infarctions, white-matter diseases, and small lesions.

The EEG is not indicated for routine evaluation of dementia because the common dementia-producing illnesses cause only slowing or minor, nonspecific abnormalities. Moreover, those EEG abnormalities are often indistinguishable from normal age-related changes. On the other hand, an EEG may help if patients have shown certain unusual clinical features, such as seizures, myoclonus, rapidly progressive dementia, or stupor. In these cases, an EEG may show indications of Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease, subacute sclerosing panencephalitis (SSPE), or delirium (see later). An EEG can also contribute to a diagnosis of depression-induced cognitive impairment where it will be normal or show only mildly slowed background activity.

Likewise, a lumbar puncture (LP) is not a routine test, but possibly helpful in certain circumstances. For example, neurologists perform the LP to test the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) when patients with dementia have indications of infectious illnesses, such as neurosyphilis, SSPE, or Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease. They also perform it to measure the pressure and withdraw CSF in cases of suspected NPH.

Physicians should reserve certain other tests for particular indications. For instance, if adolescents or young adults develop dementia, an evaluation might include serum ceruloplasmin determination and slit-lamp examination for Wilson disease; urine toxicology screens for drug abuse; and, depending on the circumstances, urine analysis for metachromatic granules and arylsulfatase-A activity for metachromatic leukodystrophy (see Chapter 5). Likewise, physicians should judiciously request serologic tests for autoimmune or inflammatory disease, serum Lyme disease titer determinations, and other tests for systemic illnesses.

Alzheimer Disease

The preliminary DSM-5 defines Mild and Major subtypes of Neurocognitive Disorder due to Alzheimer’s Disease. Its Major subtype definition relies on the clinical picture and accepts additional evidence to support the diagnosis. The Mild subtype allows the diagnosis if genetic, imaging, or other physiologic tests support it.

Neurologists have recently come to recognize three stages of Alzheimer disease, which are not exactly analogous to those in the DSM-5:

Dementia

Alzheimer disease eventually produces dementia, but at different rates and in uneven trajectories for different patients. Cognitive reserve potentially explains some of the differences. In this explanation, education, occupation, and leisure-time activities insulate the brain. Well-educated individuals, for example, continue to maintain an intellectual perspective and employ alternate psychologic strategies that allow them to cope with decline in certain domains.

In the dementia stage, patients may remain superficially conversant, sociable, able to perform routine tasks, and physically intact. Nevertheless, the spouse or caregiver will report that they suffer from incapacitating memory impairment and inability to cope with new situations. Patients’ language impairment typically includes a decrease in spontaneous verbal output, an inability to find words (anomia), use of incorrect words (paraphasic errors), and a tendency to circumvent forgotten words – elements of aphasia (see Chapter 8).

Several other symptoms stem from deterioration in visuospatial abilities. This impairment explains why patients lose their way in familiar surroundings. It also explains constructional apraxia, the inability either to translate an idea into use of a physical object or to integrate visual and motor functions.

Neuropsychiatric Manifestations

As the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) has shown, the majority of Alzheimer disease patients demonstrate apathy or agitation. In addition, many demonstrate dysphoria and abnormal behavior. Delusions, which emerge in about 20–40% of patients, are usually relatively simple, but often incorporate paranoid ideation.

Occurring about half as frequently as delusions, hallucinations are usually visual, but sometimes auditory or even olfactory. Whatever their form, hallucinations portend behavioral disturbances, rapidly progressive dementia, an unequivocally abnormal EEG, and a poor prognosis. However, their presence carries several important clinical caveats. An underlying toxic-metabolic disturbance, such as pneumonia, rather than progression of dementia, is often the cause of hallucinations. In another caveat, visual hallucinations early in the course of dementia, particularly if the patient has a parkinsonian gait, suggest a different illness – DLB (see later).

Disruptive behavior – wandering, verbal outbursts, physical agitation, and restlessness – occurs in almost 50% of Alzheimer disease patients and increases in frequency as cognition and function deteriorate. It prompts institutionalization and the problematic use of antipsychotics. Despite its dangerous aspects and association with deterioration, disruptive behavior does not increase the mortality rate.

Wandering constitutes a particularly troublesome, although not unique, manifestation of Alzheimer disease. It may originate in any combination of numerous disturbances, including memory impairment, visuospatial perceptual difficulties, delusions and hallucinations, akathisia (see Chapter 18), and mundane activities, such as looking for food.

Alzheimer patients also lose their normal circadian sleep–wake pattern to a greater degree than cognitively intact elderly people (see Chapter 17). Their sleep becomes fragmented throughout the day and night. The disruption of their sleep parallels the severity of their dementia.

Alzheimer patients’ motor vehicle accident rate is greater than comparably aged individuals, and it increases with the duration of their illness. Clues to unsafe driving include missing exits, under- or over-turning, inability to parallel park, and delayed braking. Accidents are more apt to occur on local streets than on the highway. (Yet, 16–24-year-old men have an even higher rate of motor vehicle accidents than Alzheimer patients!) Several states require that physicians report patients with medical impairments, presumably including Alzheimer disease, that interfere with safe driving. Another potentially dangerous activity of Alzheimer disease patients is that about 60% of them vote.

Physical Signs

Patients with Alzheimer disease characteristically have no physical impairment, except for one small but intriguing finding – anosmia (see Chapter 4). Even before the onset of dementia, Alzheimer disease patients require high concentrations of a volatile substance to detect and identify it. Neurologists also find anosmia in patients with Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies disease, but not in those with VCI or depression. Patients with TBI with or without dementia also have anosmia because they have had shearing of the olfactory nerve fibers at the cribriform plate (see Chapter 22).

Until Alzheimer disease advances to unequivocal dementia, patients usually remain ambulatory and able to feed themselves. The common sight of an Alzheimer disease patient walking steadily but aimlessly through a neighborhood characterizes the disparity between intellectual and motor deficits. When patients reach advanced stages of the illness, physicians can elicit frontal release signs (Fig. 7-4), increased jaw jerk reflex (see Fig. 4-13), and Babinski signs. Unlike patients with VCI, those with Alzheimer disease do not have lateralized signs. They eventually become mute, fail to respond to verbal requests, remain confined to bed, and assume a decorticate (fetal) posture. They frequently slip into a persistent vegetative state (see Chapter 11).

Laboratory Tests

When neurologists suspect Alzheimer disease, they order routine tests primarily to exclude other causes. The first EEG change is usually slowing of background activity; however, this change is not universal and overlaps the expected age-related slowing. In advanced disease, the EEG usually shows disorganized and slow background activity, which is diagnostically helpful but still nonspecific.

CT shows cerebral atrophy and a widened third ventricle, which is also suggestive but nonspecific. MRI shows sequential, progressive atrophy: first the hippocampus, then the temporal and parietal lobes, and eventually the frontal lobes. As with CT, MRI shows widening of the third ventricle as well as the atrophy. Although CT and MRI are both useful and one at least is necessary, neither can firmly diagnose Alzheimer disease because its most consistently visible abnormalities – cerebral atrophy, hippocampal atrophy, and an enlarged third ventricle – are also present in people with normal age-related changes and numerous illnesses, including trisomy 21, alcoholic dementia, HIV-associated dementia, and some varieties of schizophrenia.

In about one-half of Alzheimer patients, PET for glucose metabolism shows areas of decreased metabolism in the bilateral parietal and temporal association cortex (see Fig. 20-29). These hypometabolic areas are vague at first, but as the disease progresses, hypometabolism spreads to the frontal lobe cortex and becomes more distinct. PET remains unsuitable for routine testing, mostly because of its low sensitivity and specificity, as well as its cost. The changes are more specific and more cost-effective for frontotemporal dementia (see later). PET for amyloid accumulation is useful in the diagnosis of the preclinical stage of Alzheimer disease, but it is probably unnecessary in the symptomatic stage. Similarly, CSF analysis for depressed concentrations of amyloid and elevated concentrations of tau protein may be helpful in the preclinical stage, but superfluous in the symptomatic stage. Neurologists have not yet defined the role of PET and advanced CSF analysis in MCI.

Neurologists almost never request cerebral cortex biopsies for diagnostic purposes – mostly because routine evaluation is approximately 90% specific and sensitive.* In addition, histologic findings of Alzheimer disease differ mostly quantitatively, rather than qualitatively, from age-related changes. As a last resort, cerebral cortex biopsies can help establish a diagnosis of herpes simplex encephalitis, familial Alzheimer disease, and Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease or its variant.

Pathology

Compared to age-matched controls, brains of Alzheimer disease patients are even more atrophic. The cerebral atrophy in Alzheimer disease, although generalized, primarily affects the cortical association areas, such as the parietal–temporal junction, and the limbic system. It particularly strikes the hippocampus and prominently involves the locus ceruleus and olfactory nerve. The atrophy spares the cerebral regions governing motor, sensory, and visual functions, thus explaining the absence of paresis, sensory loss, and blindness in Alzheimer disease.

Cerebral atrophy naturally leads to compensatory dilation of the lateral and third ventricles. Of these, the temporal horns of the lateral ventricles expand the most. Nevertheless, in Alzheimer disease and most other illnesses that cause dementia, the anterior horns of the lateral ventricles maintain their concave (bowed inward) shape because of the indentation on their lateral border by the head of the preserved caudate nucleus (see Figs 20-2 and 20-18). The exception is Huntington disease, where atrophy of the head of the caudate nucleus permits the ventricle to expand laterally and assume a convex (bowed outward) shape (see Fig. 20-5).

On a histologic level, “plaques and tangles” remain the most conspicuous feature of Alzheimer disease. Although also present in normal aging and several other illnesses, in Alzheimer disease the plaques and tangles are more plentiful. Moreover, they do not distribute themselves randomly, but congregate, like the atrophy, in the cortex association areas, limbic system, and hippocampus.

The plaques, technically neuritic plaques, which are a form of senile plaques, consist of an amyloid core surrounded by abnormal axons and dendrites (neurites) that looks like a burned-out campfire. Up to 50% of the amyloid core contains an insoluble, 42-amino-acid peptide, amyloid beta-peptide () (see later). Accumulation of Aβ is necessary, but alone is insufficient for the development of Alzheimer disease.

The tangles, neurofibrillary tangles, are also necessary but insufficient. They follow a somewhat different geographic distribution than plaques. They cluster within hippocampus neurons. The tangles appear as paired, periodic helical filaments within neurons and disrupt the normal cytoskeletal architecture. They consist mostly of hyperphosphorylated forms of tau protein, which is a microtubule binding protein that undergoes conformational changes. In other words, an abnormal intraneuronal protein – tau – aggregates in Alzheimer disease and contributes to the death of critical neurons. Although the concentration of plaques correlates with dementia, the correlation between neurofibrillary tangles and dementia is stronger. On the other hand, neurofibrillary tangles occur in other conditions, including progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), frontotemporal dementia, and TBI.

Another histologic feature of Alzheimer disease is the loss of neurons in the frontal and temporal lobes. Neuron loss in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (also known as the substantia innominata), which is a group of large neurons located near the septal region beneath the globus pallidus (see Fig. 21-4), constitutes the most distinctive change. Their loss depletes cerebral acetylcholine (ACh: see later). Of all these histologic features, loss of neuron synapses correlates most closely with dementia.

Histologic examination of the majority of Alzheimer disease brains also reveals coexistent vascular disease. The reverse is also true: The brains of many VCI patients also show the histologic signs of Alzheimer disease. In other words, many cases of dementia have combinations of both pathologies.

Amyloid Deposits

Returning to the formation of Aβ, enzymes encoded on chromosome 21 cleave amyloid precursor protein (APP) to precipitate Aβ in the plaques (Fig. 7-5). Aβ differs from amyloid deposited in viscera as part of various systemic illnesses, such as multiple myeloma and amyloidosis. In Alzheimer disease, Aβ accumulates early and permanently in the cerebral cortex and vital subcortical regions.

Several lines of evidence have given rise to the amyloid cascade hypothesis or simply the amyloid hypothesis as the critical mechanism in Alzheimer disease. This hypothesis proposes that the enzymatic degradation of APP results in accumulation of Aβ, which causes inflammatory and oxidative cerebral damage. Evidence for this theory includes several powerful observations:

Biochemical Abnormalities

Under normal circumstances, neurons in the basal nucleus of Meynert synthesize ACh. Using the enzyme choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), these neurons convert acetylcoenzyme-A (acetyl-CoA) and choline to ACh:

image

Normally, neurons emanating from the basal nucleus of Meynert project upward to almost the entire cerebral cortex and the limbic system to provide cholinergic (i.e., ACh) innervation. A loss of neurons in the basal nucleus of Meynert characterizes Alzheimer disease. It leads to a marked reduction in ChAT activity, then cerebral cortex ACh concentrations, and finally cerebral cholinergic activity. As with the distribution of the macroscopic and microscopic changes in Alzheimer disease, ACh activity is particularly reduced in the cortical association areas and limbic system.

Alzheimer disease is also associated with reduced concentrations of other established or putative neurotransmitters: somatostatin, substance P, norepinephrine, vasopressin, and several other polypeptides. However, compared to the ACh loss, their concentrations are not decreased profoundly or consistently, and do not correlate with dementia.

The cholinergic hypothesis, drawn from these biochemical observations, postulates that reduced cholinergic activity causes the dementia of Alzheimer disease. In addition to the histologic and biochemical data, supporting evidence includes the finding that, even in normal individuals, blocking cerebral ACh receptors causes profound memory impairments. For example, an injection of scopolamine, which has central anticholinergic activity, induces a several-minute episode of Alzheimer-like cognitive impairment that can be reversed with physostigmine (Fig. 7-6). The cholinergic hypothesis led to the introduction of donepezil (Aricept) and other cholinesterase inhibitors in an attempt at preserving ACh activity by inactivating its metabolic enzyme, cholinesterase (see Fig. 7-6 and later).

image

FIGURE 7-6 Top, The enzyme choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) catalyzes the reaction of choline and acetyl-coenzyme A (ACoA) to form acetylcholine (ACh) in neurons of the central (CNS), peripheral (PNS), and autonomic nervous system. When released from its presynaptic neurons, ACh interacts with postsynaptic ACh receptors. Middle, However, various substances block ACh from interacting with its receptors. For example, scopolamine, which readily crosses the blood–brain barrier, blocks ACh–receptor interaction in the CNS. Likewise, atropine blocks ACh receptors, but predominantly those in the autonomic nervous system. (Unless its concentration is great, atropine does not cross the blood–brain barrier.) Bottom, Cholinesterase metabolizes ACh. Thus, enzyme degradation rather than reuptake terminates ACh activity. (In contrast, reuptake terminates most dopamine and serotonin activity.) Anticholinesterases or cholinesterase inhibitors block cholinesterase and preserve ACh concentrations. The most widely known application of this strategy is using donepezil (Aricept) and other cholinesterase inhibitors to preserve ACh activity in Alzheimer disease. Similarly, physostigmine, a powerful anticholinesterase medicine that can cross the blood–brain barrier, purportedly briefly corrects ACh deficits in tardive dyskinesia as well as Alzheimer disease (see Chapter 18). Physostigmine might also counteract excessive anticholinergic activity in tricyclic antidepressant overdose or reverse the effects of scopolamine or atropine. Using cholinesterase inhibitors is also applicable to PNS disorders. For example, edrophonium (Tensilon) and pyridostigmine (Mestinon) are anticholinesterases that restore neuromuscular junction ACh activity in myasthenia gravis (see Fig. 6-2). In the opposite situation, insecticides contain powerful anticholinesterases that create excessive ACh activity at neuromuscular junctions, which paralyzes insects by overstimulating muscle contractions.

Although the ChAT deficiency in Alzheimer disease is striking, it is not unique. Pronounced, widespread ChAT deficiencies are also present in the cortex of brains in trisomy 21, Parkinson disease, and DLB, but not in those of Huntington disease.

Risk Factors and Genetic Causes

Researchers have established several risk factors for Alzheimer disease. Living longer than 65 years is the most statistically powerful risk factor for Alzheimer disease because, beginning at that age, its incidence doubles every 5 years. Thus, the prevalence rises from 10% among individuals aged 65 years to almost 40% among individuals over 85 years.

Family history of Alzheimer disease is another well-established risk factor. Although most cases of Alzheimer disease occur sporadically, the disease develops in about 20% of patients’ offspring and 10% of second-degree relatives, but in only 5% of age-matched controls. Other risk factors include trisomy 21, several genetic mutations, and possessing Apo-E 3/4 or 4/4 allele pairs (see later). Studies have found other potential risk factors, but with influences that remain weak, inconsistent, or as yet unproven, such as myocardial infarction, elevated homocysteine levels, TBI, hypertension, hyperthyroidism, and exposure to aluminum.

Certain allele combinations of the gene coding for Apo-E, a cholesterol-carrying serum protein, confer substantial risk. Synthesized in the liver and brain, serum Apo-E binds to Aβ. Neuritic plaques accumulate the mixture. Chromosome 19 encodes the gene for Apo-E in three alleles: Apo-E2, Apo-E3, and Apo-E4. Everyone inherits one of three alleles (E2, E3, E4) from each parent, giving each person an allele pair (E2–E2, E2–E3, E2–E4, E3–E4, etc.). Approximately 10–20% of the population inherits E3–E4 or E4–E4, which are the pairs most closely associated with Alzheimer disease.

Having two E4 alleles (being homozygous for E4) – and, to a lesser degree, having one E4 allele (being heterozygous for E4) – significantly increases the risk of developing Alzheimer disease. Of individuals carrying no E4 alleles, only about 20% develop Alzheimer disease; of those carrying one E4 allele, 50% develop the disease; and of those carrying two E4 alleles, 90% develop it. Overall, more than 50% of Alzheimer disease patients carry one E4 allele. Not only does the E4 allele put the individual at risk for developing the disease, it also hastens the appearance of the symptoms. For example, compared to individuals carrying no E4 alleles, those carrying one E4 allele show Alzheimer disease symptoms 5–10 years earlier, and those with two E4 alleles show the symptoms 10–20 years earlier.

On the other hand, having one or even two E4 alleles is neither necessary nor sufficient for developing Alzheimer disease. The association is close, but not enough to be causal. Because E4 alleles remain a powerful risk factor and not a cause, neurologists refer to the Apo gene as a “susceptibility” gene. Physicians and some individuals might imagine that determining the Apo status would help diagnostically or prognostically, but the general neurologic consensus is that determining the Apo alleles provides little reliable or useful information, especially even compared to the clinical evaluation and other testing. Neurologists generally do not order Apo testing. However, patients learning that they carry Apo-E4 alleles show no significant short-term psychological risks.

Inheriting an Apo-E4 allele also serves as a marker for rapid progression from MCI to dementia in HIV disease and following severe TBI. However, the allele probably has little or no influence on the development of cognitive impairment in MS or Parkinson disease.

Buy Membership for Neurology Category to continue reading. Learn more here