Chapter 17 Clinical neuropsychology in rehabilitation
Introduction
Clinical neuropsychology was given an inevitable stimulus by the two world wars of the twentieth century, the study of missile wounds proving a fertile ground for the association of specific psychological deficits with defined regions of the brain. This research carried significant implications for a debate, inherited from nineteenth-century neurology which occupied at least the first half of the twentieth century, about the nature of the representation of psychological functions in the brain. Put rather crudely, the opposite poles of the debate argued either for the highly localized and specific representation of functions, or for a mass action view whereby psychological functions are distributed across the entire cerebral cortex. This debate has never been finally resolved, but the position that most clinical neuropsychologists now adopt is one of relative localization: that many functions are localized to regions of the cortex but cannot be more finely localized. This is often qualified by a tertiary model of cortical function in that the primary cortex, subserving sensation and discrete motor control, is quite highly localized; the secondary cortex, subserving perception and the control of movements, is rather less localized; and the tertiary or association cortex, supporting all higher-level functions, is much less clearly localized. However, current developments in connectionist theory, which point to radical models for neuropsychological processes, are starting to modify these views. For a fuller discussion, see Beaumont (1996, 2008) and for illustrations see Code et al. (1996, 2001).
Approaches in clinical neuropsychology
Neuropsychological assessment
The three traditions
There are historically three traditions in clinical neuropsychology. The first, most eloquently expressed in the work of Luria (see Christensen, 1974), is based upon behavioural neurology, although it is a much more sophisticated extension of it. The approach is based upon the presentation of simple tasks, selected in a coherent way from a wide variety of tests available, which any normal individual can be expected successfully to complete without difficulty. Any failure on the task is a pathological sign and the pattern of these signs, in skilled hands, allows a psychological description to be built up.
Cognitive functions
Even a partial description of the most popular tests is outside the scope of this chapter, but a good introduction may be found in both Halligan et al. (2003) and Goldstein and McNeil (2004), and a more thorough account in Hobben and Milberg (2002), Lezak et al. (2004) and Spreen and Strauss (2006).
Outcome measures and the quality of life
The political climate of health service changes in the UK has forced health-care providers to consider the outcome of their interventions, and this can only be to the advantage of clients. Psychologists, because of their expertise in the measurement of behaviour, have been prominent in the development of outcome measures. Within neuropsychological rehabilitation there is a variety of measures, of which the Barthel Index (Wade, 1992) is widely used, and FIM–FAM (Functional Independence Measure–Functional Assessment Measure; Cook et al., 1994; Ditunno, 1992) is growing in popularity as it can be linked to problem-oriented and client-centred rehabilitation planning. However, none of the available scales is adequate to assess the status of severely disabled clients (Stokes, 2009), and there is also a lack of good measures of the specific outcome of psychological interventions. Research is actively being undertaken to fill these gaps, and a discussion is to be found in Fleminger and Powell (1999).
Neuropsychological interventions
Management strategies include cognitive and behavioural interventions as well as psychotherapy.
Cognitive interventions
Besides the explicit teaching of new strategies, appropriately structured training may be employed; this is often based upon ‘error-free learning’, which has been shown to be most effective following head injury. Aids to performance, which may be either external (such as diaries to aid memory) or internal (mnemonics), may also be successfully employed. These interventions are more fully developed in some areas than others, and have been used most extensively in the rehabilitation of memory (Baddeley et al., 2002; Wilson & Moffat, 1992), but the basic principles can be applied in any area of cognitive function (see also Ponsford, 1995; Prigatano, 1999; Riddoch & Humphreys, 1994; Sohlberg & Mateer, 2002).
Behavioural interventions
The range of behavioural techniques is both wider and more sophisticated than this brief account might suggest, and in certain selected contexts these approaches may be highly effective (Wood, 1990). However, the demands on resources and staff skills are high, given that a behavioural programme must be applied consistently, contingently and continuously, often over a very protracted period. For this reason, behavioural approaches are less commonly employed outside specialist facilities, although they are perhaps unreasonably neglected within other rehabilitation contexts.
Neuropsychological consequences of neurological disorders
The consequences and management of neuropsychological problems cannot be discussed in any detail in this chapter, but several useful texts exist (e.g. Beaumont, 2008; Beaumont et al 1996; Halligan et al., 2003; Kolb & Whishaw, 2003).
General considerations
Acute versus chronic
Acute lesions have greater effects than chronic lesions. In the acute period following the acquisition of a lesion there may be widespread disruption of psychological functions, together with changes in the level of consciousness, confusion and loss of orientation. Amnesia is common in the acute period, and the duration of post-traumatic amnesia, before continuous memory and full orientation return, is the best indicator of the severity of the lesion (see Ch. 3). Neuropsychological consequences diminish over time, most of the recovery occurring within the first 6–12 months, but with further improvements occurring over the next year or a little longer.
Site and lateralization
The site is obviously of relevance in the case of a focal lesion, and will determine the neuropsychological consequences within the principle of relative localization (see above). Lateralization, whether the lesion is primarily located in the left or right hemisphere of the cerebral cortex, is also of relevance as the psychological functions assumed by the two hemispheres are known to differ. There is an enormous literature on cerebral lateralization, which was the most prominent research topic of neuropsychology for the two decades from about 1960, and most functions show some degree of differential lateralization. The clearest case is speech, which is exclusively located in the left hemisphere of about 95% of right-handed individuals (Beaumont et al., 1996; Kolb & Whishaw, 2003).
Age of acquisition
The age at which a lesion is acquired may also be of relevance, as the effects are less in the younger patient and throughout the childhood years (the Kennard principle). This was previously attributed to an increased ‘plasticity’ of the developing brain, in that alternative regions could subsume the functions previously destined to be located in the area containing the lesion. However, there is now some doubt over this hypothesis, partly due to accumulating evidence for the continuing neural adaptability of the brain in adult life (DeFlna et al., 2009; Hoffman & Harrison, 2009; Stein & Hoffman, 2003). The explanation may lie at least as much in the cognitive flexibility of the developing psychological systems and the greater opportunities for alternative forms of learning in the pre-adult period.
Specific aetiologies
As stated above, it is not possible to describe the management of neuropsychological problems in the conditions mentioned in this section. As well as the psychology texts cited in this chapter, the reader is referred to a book on neurological rehabilitation which devotes sections to cognitive and behavioural problems (Greenwood et al., 2003).
Head injury
Head injury is the most common cause of neurodisability for which rehabilitation is undertaken (see Ch. 3). It affects young males more than any other group and effective intervention can result in a very favourable outcome. Head injuries range from very mild to very severe and profound, with dramatic differences in the behavioural consequences up to and including prolonged coma and vegetative states. The lesions associated with head injury are generally focal or multifocal, and static, although acceleration–deceleration closed head injuries may result in widespread and diffuse lesions across the cortex. An important consideration is that even apparently very mild head injuries associated with a brief period of concussion may sometimes have significant behavioural consequences in terms of anxiety, depression, changes in personality and subtle disorders of memory, with consequent effects upon occupational performance, social activity and personal relationships.
Stroke
Stroke, perhaps because it tends to occur in the more elderly, has received less attention than head injury. The effects of strokes and other cerebrovascular accidents will depend upon the area and proportion of the arterial distribution which is lost, ranging from the whole territory of one of the main cerebral arteries, which is a substantial proportion of the cortex, down to relatively discrete focal lesions associated with a distal portion of one of these arteries (see Ch. 2). Although the neuropsychological consequences depend primarily on the area of cortex affected, the picture is often complicated by the occurrence of further, perhaps minor, strokes that prevent the psychological condition being stable, and by associated arterial disease, which may result in more general and perhaps fluctuating insufficiency of the blood supply to the entire cortex.
Degenerative conditions
Interest in the degenerative conditions from a neuropsychological perspective has grown in recent years. Other than the dementias of later life, principally dementia of the Alzheimer type, which are clearly associated with deficits of cognitive function of a progressive nature, there are a number of other degenerative conditions which occur in adult life, but of which the neuropsychological consequences are only beginning to be understood. Multiple sclerosis, the most common neurological disease of the population, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease and motor neurone disease, sometimes referred to collectively as the subcortical dementias, are all associated with cognitive, affective and behavioural deficits in a significant proportion of those with the disease, and almost all those whose disease progresses to an advanced stage suffer psychological sequelae (see Chs 5–8). Disorders of memory, attention and affect are common as primary consequences of the disease in this group, and there are naturally significant psychological disturbances associated with being a sufferer of one of these diseases.
Spinal injuries
Spinal cord injury clearly differs from other neurological conditions in that the patient has suffered a disabling condition, but all neural systems supporting psychological functions are intact (see Ch. 4). The main issue is therefore one of adjustment to the disability, both in terms of the primary impairment dependent upon the spinal cord lesion, and the secondary consequences of handicap that follow from the disability. Amongst the primary disabilities are loss of mobility and other functional capacities (especially if the upper limbs are affected), together with loss of bladder and bowel control, and, most importantly for psychological health, sexual function may also be affected. Depression is very common following spinal injury, and the facilitation of insight and adjustment to the disability is a primary task for the neuropsychologist.
Concluding issues
Psychological adjustment
Good psychological adjustment depends upon:
Baddeley A.D., Kopelman M., Wilson B.A., editors. The handbook of memory disorders for clinicians, second ed, Chichester: John Wiley, 2002.
Beaumont J.G. Introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Guilford Press, 2008.
Beaumont J.G. Neuropsychology. In: Beaumont J.G., Kenealy P.M., Rogers M.J.C., editors. Blackwell dictionary of neuropsychology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers; 1996:523-531.
Beaumont J.G., Kenealy P.M., Rogers M.J.C., editors. Blackwell dictionary of neuropsychology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 1996.
Christensen A.L. Luria’s neuropsychological investigation. Munksgaard: Copenhagen, 1974.
Code C., Wallesch C.W., Lecours A.R., Joanette Y. Classic cases in neuropsychology. Hove: Psychology Press, 1996.
Code C., Wallesch C.W., Joanette Y., Lecours A.R. Classic cases in neuropsychology, II. Hove: Psychology Press. 2001.
Cook L., Smith D.S., Truman G. Using Functional Independence Measure profiles as an index of outcome in the rehabilitation of brain-injured patients. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil.. 1994;75:390-393.
DeFlna P., Fellus J., Polito M.Z., Thompson J.W., Moser R.S., DeLuca J. The new neuroscience frontier: promoting neuroplasticity and brain repair in traumatic brain injury. J. Clin. Neuropsychol.. 2009;23(8):1391-1399.
Ditunno J.F.Jr. Functional assessment measures in CNS trauma. J. Neurotrauma. 1992;9(Suppl. 1):S301-S305.
Fleminger S., Powell J., editors. Evaluation of outcomes in brain injury rehabilitation. Hove: Psychology Press, 1999.
Goldstein L.H., McNeil J.E. Clinical neuropsychology: A practical guide to assessment and management for clinicians. Chichester: John Wiley, 2004.
Greenwood R., Barnes M.P., McMillan T.M., et al, editors. Handbook of neurological rehabilitation. London: Psychology Press, 2003.
Halligan P.W., Kischka U., Marshall J.C., editors. Handbook of clinical neuropsychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2003.
Hobben N., Milberg W. Essentials of neuropsychological assessment. New York: Wiley, 2002.
Hoffman S.W., Harrison C. The interaction between psychological health and traumatic brain injury: a neuroscience perspective. J. Clin. Neuropsychol.. 2009;23(8):1400-1415.
Kolb B., Whishaw I.Q. Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, fifth ed. New York: WH Freeman and Co, 2003.
Lezak M., Howieson D.B., Loring D.W., et al. Neuropsychological assessment, fourth ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 2004.
Ponsford J. Traumatic brain injury; Rehabilitation for everyday adaptive living. Hove: Psychology Press, 1995.
Prigatano G. Principles of neuropsychological rehabilitation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999.
Riddoch M.J., Humphreys G.W. Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc, 1994.
Sohlberg M.M., Mateer C.A. Cognitive rehabilitation: An integrative neuropsychological approach. New York: Guilford Press, 2002.
Spreen O., Strauss E.A. compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary, third ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 2006.
Stein D.G., Hoffman S.W. Concepts of CNS plasticity in the context of brain damage and repair. J. Head Trauma Rehabil.. 2003;18(4):317-341.
Stokes E.K. Outcome measurement. In: Lennon S., Stokes M., editors. Pocketbook of Neurological Physiotherapy. Edinburgh. Churchill Livingstone: Elsevier; 2009:191-201.
Wade D.T. The Barthel ADL index: guidelines. In: Wade D.T., editor. Measurement in neurological rehabilitation. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1992:177-178.
Wilson B.A., Moffat N. Clinical management of memory problems, second ed. London: Chapman & Hall, 1992.
Wood R.L. Neurobehavioural sequelae of traumatic brain injury. London: Taylor & Francis, 1990.