Aphasia and Anosognosia

Published on 03/03/2015 by admin

Filed under Neurology

Last modified 22/04/2025

Print this page

rate 1 star rate 2 star rate 3 star rate 4 star rate 5 star
Your rating: none, Average: 5 (1 votes)

This article have been viewed 3317 times

Chapter 8 Aphasia and Anosognosia

Neurologists utilize the anatomic and physiologic correlates of language, language impairment (aphasia), and related disorders to deduce how the normal brain functions and to advance linguistics studies. They test for language-related disorders, often quite striking in their presentation, to help localize and diagnose neurologic disease.

Aphasia, the flagship of neuropsychologic disturbances, can disrupt cognition, halt certain functions, and produce mental aberrations. Aphasia and its related disorders appear prominently in many neurologic and psychiatric disorders.

Language and Dominance

The dominant hemisphere, by definition, governs language function and houses the brain’s language centers. In its association areas, the dominant hemisphere also integrates language with intellect, emotion, and somatic, auditory, and visual sensations. Because of these crucial roles, the dominant hemisphere serves as the brain’s main portal for expression of cognitive activity and emotions.

Language development begins in infancy, which is also the period of greatest brain plasticity (ability to be remodeled). By 5 years of age, the brain establishes dominance for language. Afterwards, as vocabulary, verbal nuance, and intellectual complexity increase, plasticity declines. For example, once through puberty, children usually cannot learn new (second) languages without preserving traces of their native (primary) language. Also after puberty, the nondominant hemisphere can no longer assume a meaningful role in language following injury of the dominant hemisphere.

Language includes more than speaking, listening, reading, and writing. It also includes sign language and tone-dependent languages, such as several Chinese dialects. The dominant hemisphere’s perisylvian language arc (see later) processes all forms of language. Thus, lesions in this region impair, to a greater or lesser extent, all language forms. Most patients with a dominant-hemisphere stroke, for example, exhibit deficits in reading that parallel deficits in writing. (One notable exception consists of alexia without agraphia [see later].)

Although the dominant hemisphere controls languages learned in infancy, it does not necessarily control those learned as adults, including a second language, or use of obscenities, which is usually an expression of strong emotions. The nondominant hemisphere bestows on spoken words their prosody, which consists of their inflection, rhythm, and manner that determines their affect and shades their meaning.

Cerebral hemisphere dominance includes more than control of language. The dominant hemisphere also controls fine, rapid hand movements (handedness) and, to a lesser degree, reception of vision and hearing. For example, right-handed people, who almost always have left cerebral hemisphere dominance (see later), not only rely on their right hand for writing and throwing a ball. They also rely on their right foot for kicking, right eye when peering through a telescope, and right ear for listening to words spoken simultaneously in both ears (dichotic listening).

The dominant hemisphere is also distinctive in its exception to the general left–right anatomic symmetry of the brain. The dominant temporal lobe’s superior surface – the planum temporale – has a much greater cortical area than its nondominant counterpart because it has more gyri and deeper sulci. Not only does the relatively large cortical area of the dominant planum temporale provide greater language capacity, it probably also allows for greater musical ability because it is larger in musicians than nonmusicians, and largest in musicians with perfect pitch. However, this normal asymmetry is lacking or even reversed in many individuals with dyslexia, autism, and chronic schizophrenia – conditions with prominent language abnormalities.

Handedness

About 90% of all people are right-handed and correspondingly left-hemisphere-dominant. In addition, most left-handed people are also left-hemisphere-dominant.

Although truly left-handed people naturally tend to have right hemisphere dominance, for some of them congenital injury to their left hemisphere had forced their right hemisphere to assume dominance. In addition, left-handedness is over-represented among individuals with overt neurologic impairment, such as mental retardation or epilepsy, and certain major psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and autism. Moreover, it is over-represented among children with subtle neurologic abnormalities, such as dyslexia and other learning disabilities, stuttering, and general clumsiness.

Left-handed people are also disproportionately represented among musicians, artists, mathematicians, athletes, and recent US presidents. (In the last several decades, left-handed presidents have included Gerald Ford, George H. Bush, Bill Clinton, Barack Obama, and, on most occasions, Ronald Reagan, but right-handed presidents numbered only two: Jimmy Carter and George W. Bush.)

Left-handed athletes tend to perform better than right-handed ones, but only in sports involving direct confrontation, such as baseball, tennis, fencing, and boxing, because they benefit from certain tactical advantages, such as a left-handed batter being closer to first base. They achieve no greater success in sports without direct confrontation, such as swimming, running, pole-vaulting, and other track and field events.

Unlike right-handed individuals, left-handed ones become aphasic after injury to either cerebral hemisphere. In addition, if left-handed individuals develop aphasia, its variety relates less closely to the specific injury site (see later) and their prognosis is better than if right-handed individuals develop a comparable aphasia.

Many ambidextrous individuals presumably have mixed dominance. Seemingly endowed with language, music, and motor skill function in both hemispheres, they tend to excel in sports and performing on musical instruments.

Although the left hemisphere is dominant in approximately 95% of all people (and the rest of this chapter assumes it is always the case), sometimes neurologists must establish dominance with certainty. For example, when neurosurgeons must resect a portion of the dominant temporal lobe because of a tumor or intractable partial complex epilepsy (see Chapter 10), they can resect only a limited, carefully mapped area. Resecting too large or the wrong area might be complicated by devastating aphasia or memory impairment.

Using the Wada test – essentially injections of amobarbital directly into a carotid artery – neurologists can establish cerebral dominance. When the amobarbital perfuses the dominant hemisphere, it renders the patient temporarily aphasic. Similarly, infusion of amobarbital into one temporal lobe may cause temporary amnesia if the other temporal lobe was previously damaged. Another diagnostic test, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), in which subjects essentially undergo language evaluation during an MRI, is potentially more reliable, clearly safer, and easier to perform.

Aphasia

The Perisylvian Language Arc

Impulses conveying speech, music, and simple sounds travel from the ears along the acoustic (eighth cranial) nerves into the brainstem, where they synapse in the medial geniculate body. Then postsynaptic, crossed and uncrossed brainstem tracts bring the impulses to the primary auditory cortex, Heschl’s gyri, in each temporal lobe (see Fig. 4-16). Most music and some other sounds remain based in the nondominant hemisphere. In contrast, the brain transmits language impulses to Wernicke’s area, which is situated in the dominant temporal lobe. From there, they travel in the arcuate fasciculus, coursing posteriorly through the temporal and parietal lobes, and then looping anteriorly to the frontal lobe’s Broca’s area. This vital language center – located immediately anterior to the motor center for the right face, larynx, pharynx, and arm (Fig. 8-1) – receives processed, integrated language impulses, converts them to speech, and activates the adjacent motor cortex. A horseshoe-shaped region of cerebral cortex, which surrounds the sylvian fissure, the perisylvian language arc, contains Wernicke’s area, the arcuate fasciculus, and Broca’s area.

Using the perisylvian language arc model, researchers have established normal and abnormal language patterns. For example, when normal people repeat aloud what they hear, auditory impulses go first to Wernicke’s area, then pass around the arcuate fasciculus, and finally arrive in Broca’s area for speech production (Fig. 8-2, A). Reading aloud is a complicated variation of repeating aloud because reading requires both hemispheres and a learned system of decoding symbols, i.e., having been taught to read. As people read, their geniculocalcarine pathway transmits visual impulses from the lateral geniculate bodies to the calcarine (visual) cortex in both the left and right occipital lobes (see Fig. 4-1). Impulses from the left visual field go to the right occipital cortex. Then those impulses must travel through the posterior corpus callosum to reach the left (dominant) cerebral hemisphere. The impulses that have crossed from the right visual cortex merge with those already in the left hemisphere’s parietal lobe. Decoded, coherent language information then travels from the left parietal lobe via the arcuate fasciculus to Broca’s area for articulation (Fig. 8-2, B).

image

FIGURE 8-2 A, When people repeat aloud, language information arrives in Wernicke’s area, located adjacent to Heschl’s gyrus (see Fig. 4-16), and then travels through the parietal lobe in the arcuate fasciculus to Broca’s area. This area innervates the adjacent cerebral cortex for the tongue, lips, larynx, and pharynx. B, When people read aloud, visual impulses are received by the left and right occipital visual cortex regions. Both regions send impulses to a left parietal lobe association region (the oval), which converts text to language. Impulses from the left visual field, which are initially received in the right cortex, must first pass through the posterior corpus callosum to reach the language centers (see Fig. 8-4).

Language, of course, does not stand isolated from the brain’s other neuropsychologic functions. The language arc maintains reciprocal connections with cerebral cortical areas for memory, emotion, and similar domains. It also has strong connections with the thalamus, basal ganglia, and other subcortical structures.

Clinical Evaluation

Before diagnosing aphasia, the clinician must keep in mind normal language variations. Normal individuals may struggle and stammer when confronted with a novel experience, particularly a neurologic examination. Many have their own style and rhythm. Some may be reticent, uneducated, intimidated, or hostile. Others, before speaking, consider each word and formulate every phrase as though carefully considering which item to choose from a menu, but some blurt out the first thing on their mind.

In diagnosing aphasia, the clinician can use various classifications. A favorite distinguishes receptive (sensory) from expressive (motor) aphasia based on relative impairment of verbal reception versus expression. However, a major drawback of that classification is that most aphasic patients have a mixture of impairments that do not permit specific diagnosis.

The most useful classification of the aphasias, nonfluent/fluent, rests on the quantity of the patient’s verbal output (Table 8-1A). It suffices for clinical evaluations and roughly correlates with imaging studies. Aphasia aficionados subdivide nonfluent aphasia and fluent aphasia each into four categories based primarily on the language examination showing the presence or absence of the patient’s ability to comprehend, repeat, and name objects. When repetition ability remains intact in either nonfluent or fluent category, neurologists add the designation transcortical.

TABLE 8-1A Salient Features of the Nonfluent and Fluent Aphasias

Feature Nonfluent Fluent
Other terms Expressive Receptive
  Motor Sensory
  Broca’s Wernicke’s
Spontaneous Speech Nonverbal Verbal
Content Paucity of words, mostly nouns and verbs Complete sentences with normal syntax
Articulation Dysarthric, slow, stuttering Good
Errors Telegraphic speech Paraphasic errors, circumlocutions, tangentialities, clang associations
Associated deficits Right hemiparesis (arm, face > leg) Hemianopsia, hemisensory loss
Localization of lesion Frontal lobe Temporal or parietal lobe
    Occasionally diffuse

Clinicians usually detect aphasia in a patient during the introductory conversation, history taking, or mental status examination. They perform a standard series of simple verbal tests to identify and classify the aphasia. The tests systematically evaluate three basic language functions: comprehension, naming, and repetition (Box 8-1). Depending on the clinical situation, the examiner may request increasingly difficult levels of comprehension, more uncommon objects, or more complicated phrases. The examiner may also perform the same testing with written requests and responses; however, with one notable exception, alexia without agraphia (see later), defects in written communication parallel those in verbal communication.

Nonfluent Aphasia

Characteristics

Paucity of speech characterizes nonfluent aphasia. Patients say little and usually only in response to direct questions. Whatever speech they produce consists almost exclusively of single words and short phrases. They rely on basic words, particularly nouns and verbs. They cannot use the connective tissue of language: adjectives, adverbs, and conjunctions. Their longer phrases typically consist entirely of stock phrases or sound bites, such as, “Not so bad” or “Get out of here.”

Patients are, to a greater or lesser degree, nonverbal. Their speech typically contains less than 50 words per minute, which is much slower than normal (100–150 words per minute). They produce it in a slow and effortful manner.

Another hallmark of nonfluent speech is that excessive pauses interrupt its flow. Neurologists sometimes describe its jerky tempo as “telegraphic.” For example, in response to a question about food, a patient might stammer “fork . . . steak . . eat . . . . no.”

Depending on the variety of nonfluent aphasia, patients cannot repeat simple phrases or name common objects. In contrast, most patients with nonfluent aphasia retain relatively normal comprehension that can be illustrated by their ability to follow verbal requests, such as “Please, close your eyes” or “Raise your left hand, please.”

The four major subdivisions of nonfluent aphasia are the following (Table 8-1B):

TABLE 8-1B Nonfluent Aphasias

  Comprehension Repetition
Broca’s Intact Lost
Transcortical motor Intact Intact
Mixed transcortical Lost Intact
(isolation)    
Global Lost Lost

Localization and Etiology

Lesions responsible for nonfluent aphasias usually encompass, surround, or sit near Broca’s area (Fig. 8-3, A). The etiology is usually a middle cerebral artery stroke or other discrete structural lesion. Their location, not their pathology, produces the aphasia. Whatever the etiology, these lesions tend to be so extensive that they damage neighboring structures, such as the motor cortex and posterior sensory cortex. Moreover, because they are usually spherical or conical, rather than superficial two-dimensional lesions, they damage underlying white-matter tracts, including the visual pathway. Diffuse cerebral injuries, such as anoxia, metabolic disturbances or Alzheimer disease, rarely cause nonfluent aphasia.

Neurologists originally labeled nonfluent aphasia “expressive” or “motor” because of the prominent speech impairment. They also labeled all nonfluent aphasias “Broca’s” because of the responsible lesion’s location.

Associated Deficits

Because the lesion causing nonfluent aphasia usually damages the motor cortex and other adjacent regions, right hemiparesis usually accompanies this aphasia. In such cases, the hemiparesis affects the arm and lower face, and causes poor articulation (dysarthria). Lesions with any depth also cause a right homonymous hemianopsia (visual field cut). One of the most common syndromes in neurology is an occlusion of the left middle cerebral artery producing the combination of nonfluent aphasia and right-sided hemiparesis and homonymous hemianopsia.

Another nonlanguage consequence of the lesions is buccofacial apraxia, also called “oral apraxia.” This apraxia, like others, is not paresis or any kind of involuntary movement disorder, but the inability to execute normal voluntary movements of the face, lip, and tongue. When buccofacial apraxia occurs in conjunction with nonfluent aphasia, it adds to the dysarthria.

To test for buccofacial apraxia, the clinician might ask patients to say, “La . . Pa . . La . . Pa . . La . . Pa;” protrude their tongue in different directions; and pretend to blow out a match and suck through a straw. Patients with buccofacial apraxia will be unable to comply, but they may be able to use the same muscles reflexively or when provided with cues. For example, patients who cannot speak might sing, and those who cannot pretend to use a straw might be able to suck water through an actual one.

When strokes or head trauma cause aphasia, patients improve to a greater or lesser extent. Presumably, ischemic areas of the brain recover and surviving neurons form new connections.

Mixed Transcortical or Isolation Aphasia

Some lesions, which must be diffuse and extensive, damage the cerebral cortex surrounding the language arc. By sparing the language pathway, these lesions leave basic language function intact but removed from other cognitive functions. In mixed transcortical or isolation aphasia, which stems from such a cerebral injury, patients retain their ability to repeat whatever they hear; however, they cannot interact in a conversation, follow requests, or name objects. Because they can characteristically only duplicate long strings of syllables, neurologists consider them to be nonfluent.

The signature of isolation aphasia is this disparity between patients’ seeming muteness and their preserved ability to repeat long and complex sentences. Patients who display such repetition, echolalia, mindlessly reiterate visitors’ words readily, involuntarily, and sometimes compulsively. A cursory evaluation could understandably confuse this disturbance with irrational jargon.

Damage to the entire remaining cortex usually causes cognitive impairment, usually to the point of dementia. It also usually causes decorticate posture (see Fig. 11-5).

The etiology of this aphasia usually stems from the loss of the precarious blood supply of the cerebral cortex. While major branches of left middle cerebral artery perfuse the perisylvian arc, only thin, fragile, distal branches of middle, anterior, and posterior cerebral arteries perfuse its border with the surrounding cortex (the watershed area). When these vessels deliver insufficient cerebral blood flow to this portion of the cortex, it suffers a watershed infarction (Fig. 8-3, D). Thus, cardiac or respiratory arrest, suicide attempts using carbon monoxide, and other hypotensive or hypoxic episodes cause isolation aphasia. When Alzheimer disease strikes cerebral areas other than the language and motor regions, which is uncommon, it too can cause isolation aphasia.

Fluent Aphasia

The four major subdivisions of fluent aphasia are the following (Table 8-1C):

Table 8-1C Fluent Aphasias

  Comprehension Repetition
Wernicke’s Lost Lost
Transcortical sensory Lost Intact
Conduction Intact Lost
Anomic Intact Intact

Of them, Wernicke’s is the epitome and most common. Its identifying characteristic is paraphasic errors or paraphasias, which are incorrect, meaningless, or even nonsensical words. Patients insert paraphasias into relatively complete, well-articulated, grammatically correct sentences that are spoken at a normal rate. However, paraphasias may render their conversation unintelligible. Moreover, patients typically cannot fully comprehend language or repeat simple phrases. Patients with its less severe variant, transcortical sensory aphasia, can repeat phrases, but otherwise their language impediments are similar.

Paraphasias most often consist of a word substitution, such as “clock” for “watch” or “spoon” for “fork” (related paraphasias), in which the substitute word arises from the same category. Less commonly, the words involved have little relation, such as “glove” for “knife” (unrelated paraphasia); or a nonspecific relation, such as “that” for any object (generic substitution). Paraphasias may also consist of altered words, such as “breat” for “bread” (literal paraphasia).

Paraphasias also include nonsensical coinages (neologisms), such as “I want to fin gunt in the fark.” Patient can bounce from one word to another with a close sound, but one with little or no shared meaning (clang associations, from the German klang, sound). For example, a patient making a clang association might ask, “What’s for dinner, diner, slimmer, finner?”

As if to circumvent their word-finding difficulty, fluent aphasia patients often speak in circumlocutions. They also tend toward tangential diversions or tangentialities, as though once having spoken the wrong word, they pursue the idea triggered by their error. These patients, caught in a tangentiality, may string together meaningfully related words until they reach an absurd point. For example, when attempting to name a pencil, the patient said, “pen … pence … paper … papal…”

Despite their loss of verbal communication, patients’ nonverbal expressions are preserved because nondominant-hemisphere functions remain unaffected. For example, prosody remains consistent with patients’ mood. Patients continue to express their feelings through facial gestures, body movements, and cursing. Similarly, most patients retain their ability to produce a melody even though they may be unable to repeat the lyrics. For example, patients might hum a tune, such as “Jingle Bells,” but if they attempt to sing it, paraphasias crop up in the middle of the lyrics.

Localization and Etiology

Discrete structural lesions, such as small strokes, in the temporoparietal region are the usual cause of Wernicke aphasia and fluent aphasias in general (Figs 8-3, B and 20-16). In addition, neurodegenerative illnesses, particularly Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementia (see Chapter 7), often cause fluent aphasia among other symptoms.

Mental Abnormalities with Language Impairment

Dementia

Although aphasia by itself is not equivalent to dementia, it can be a component of dementia or it can mimic dementia. For example, when aphasia impairs routine communications – saying the date and place, repeating a series of numbers, and following requests – it mimics dementia. At times, patients with severe aphasia seem so bizarre that they appear incoherent. Because people think in words, aphasia also clouds cognition and memory.

Dementia and aphasia also differ in their time course. Dementia develops slowly, but aphasia begins abruptly, except in the unusual case when it heralds a neurodegenerative illness. Nonfluent aphasia further differs from dementia in its accompanying physical aspects: dysarthria and obvious lateralized signs, such as a right-sided hemiparesis and homonymous hemianopsia. Moreover, paraphasias frequently occur in fluent aphasia but rarely in dementia.

Nevertheless, patients occasionally have both aphasia and dementia. This combination often occurs with one or more strokes superimposed on Alzheimer disease and with frontotemporal dementia. These situations defy classification because aphasia usually invalidates standard cognitive tests.

Distinguishing aphasia from dementia and recognizing when the two conditions coexist are more than an academic exercise. A diagnosis of aphasia almost always suggests that a patient has had a discrete dominant cerebral hemisphere injury. Because a stroke or other structural lesion would be the most likely cause, the appropriate evaluation would include computed tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). In contrast, a diagnosis of dementia suggests that the most likely cause would be Alzheimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, or another diffuse neurodegenerative illness, and the evaluation might include various blood tests as well as CT or MRI.

Other Disorders

Children with autism spectrum disorders may demonstrate language impairment – not only in their verbal expression, but also in their facial and bodily communication, such as failure to point. In many cases, nonsensical repetitions (stereotypies), idiosyncrasies, and echolalia overwhelm their speech. These children often fail to appreciate the nuance and affective components of language. In girls with Rett syndrome, language begins to regress after several years of normal development (see Chapter 13). Despite the possibility of a neurologic condition being responsible, when language regression appears in children, particularly boys and those younger than 3 years, they face a high probability of having an autistic disorder.

Mutism and other apparent language abnormalities are frequently manifestations of psychogenic disturbances (see Chapter 3). In these cases, apparent language impairment is usually inconsistent and amenable to suggestion. Acquired stuttering also often indicates a psychogenic impairment. For example, a patient with psychogenic aphasia might stutter and seem to be at a loss for words, but communicate normally by writing. An amobarbital infusion during an interview might reveal perfectly intact language function.

A common psychogenic aphasia-like condition is the sudden, unexpected difficulty in recalling the name (blocking) of someone who stirred a strong emotional response. The classic aphasia-like condition remains the Freudian slip, technically known as a parapraxis. Freud’s work on aphasia, which presaged his exploration of the unconscious, described his view of language circuitry and then words spoken in “error” due to repressed wish or conflict. Depending on their viewpoint, clinicians assessing everyday word substitutions may term them either paraphasias or insights into the unconscious. For example, when a physician’s former secretary, suspected of harboring a neurologic disorder, says that she has been Dr. So-and-So’s “medical cemetery,” a clinician could interpret the comment as her feelings about the competence of the doctor, an indication of the patient’s own fears of death, or a paraphasia referable to a dominant-hemisphere lesion.

Disorders Related to Aphasia

Dyslexia

In most cases, reading impairment despite normal or near normal intelligence and education represents a developmental disorder, developmental dyslexia (Greek, lexis, word or phrase). When tested, approximately 10% of all schoolchildren display some degree of developmental dyslexia. Moreover, alone or comorbid with related problems, developmental dyslexia occurs in 80% of all children with learning disabilities. Teachers usually detect it when children first try to read, but occasionally mild forms escape detection until high-school or college students confront complicated reading tasks. In about 25% of children and, to a lesser extent, in some adults, dyslexia is comorbid with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Regardless of when it first appears and acknowledging that some educational strategies ameliorate the problem, developmental dyslexia persists throughout life. For dyslexic students who are otherwise bright, studying mathematics and science helps circumvent the disability.

Dyslexia affects boys with disproportionate severity and frequency, such that the boy : girl ratio lies between 2 : 1 and 5 : 1. Many children come from families where several other members also have dyslexia. In them, studies have implicated autosomal dominant and sex-linked genes. Imaging and pathologic studies reveal that the brains of some dyslexic individuals lack the normal planum temporale asymmetry. In other words, their brains are symmetric, which is abnormal.

In older children and adults, strokes, trauma, or other lesions can produce dyslexia or completely impair reading ability. This form of alexia, acquired alexia, in contrast to developmental alexia, is usually a component of aphasia and accompanied by right-sided motor deficits. More importantly, with one notable exception (see later), agraphia (inability to write) invariably accompanies acquired alexia.

Alexia and Agraphia

In the exception, alexia without agraphia (Fig. 8-4), patients demonstrate little or no impairment in comprehending speech or expressing themselves verbally or by writing; however, they simply cannot read. For example, patients can transcribe another person’s dictation and write their own thoughts, but then be unable to read their own handwriting. Alexia without agraphia, which should really be called “alexia with graphia,” results from a destructive lesion encompassing the dominant (left) occipital lobe and adjacent posterior corpus callosum. Aside from having a right homonymous hemianopsia, patients lack physical deficits.

Apraxia

Apraxia, the motor system’s rough equivalent of aphasia, is inability to execute learned actions despite normal strength, sensation, and coordination. Neurologists attribute apraxia to disruption of links between the cerebrum’s motor and neuropsychologic centers, particularly the perisylvian language arc and frontal lobe executive centers.

Although apraxia can be readily differentiated from simple paresis, it is often comorbid with aphasia or dementia. In fact, apraxia often appears as a symptom of Alzheimer disease and other cortical dementias (see Chapter 7).

In assessing patients for apraxia, the examiner usually first tests their buccofacial (lips, face, tongue) and limb movements when they make gestures or perform “symbolic acts” (Table 8-2). Next, the examiner asks them to perform imagined actions (pantomime), first on pretend objects and then on actual ones. After seeing the examiner perform an action, patients with apraxia typically can copy it. For example, a patient with apraxia might not be able to follow the request “Please, pretend to salute an officer,” but after the examiner demonstrates the salute, the patient will duplicate it. Similarly, when patients with apraxia are handed an actual object, which gives them a cue, they can often perform the object’s intended action. For example, a patient with apraxia might not be able to pretend to use a comb, but when presented with one, the patient will use it appropriately.

As a general rule, inability to use a common tool, such as a comb or spoon, most reliably demonstrates apraxia. Further testing, depending on circumstances, includes performing a series of steps, copying figures, arranging matchsticks, walking, or dressing.

Patients typically remain unaware of their apraxia because they usually do not spontaneously attempt the various tests, such as saluting an unseen officer or using an imaginary screwdriver. Moreover, an unsophisticated clinician might incorrectly attribute the impairment to paresis or incoordination.

Despite its complexity, neurologists designate several clinically useful categories of apraxia. Ideomotor apraxia, the most common category, consists basically of the inability to convert an idea into an action. For example, patients with ideomotor apraxia cannot pantomime despite possessing a clear understanding and retaining the physical ability to comply. Clinicians might envision ideomotor apraxia as the result of a disconnection between cognitive or language regions and motor regions (Fig. 8-5). Almost invariably, a left-sided frontal or parietal lobe lesion gives rise to the apraxia. Thus, ideomotor apraxia often coexists with aphasia, particularly nonfluent aphasia, and inability of the right hand to pantomime.

One of its two varieties, buccofacial apraxia, as previously discussed, is a feature of nonfluent aphasia. In the other variety, limb apraxia, patients cannot execute simple requests usually involving their right arm or leg. They cannot pretend to brush their teeth, turn a key, comb their hair, or kick a ball. When asked to pretend to use an object, these patients characteristically use their hand as though it were the actual object. For example, they will brush their teeth with their forefinger instead of pretending to hold a toothbrush.

In ideational apraxia, patients cannot conceive and then perform a sequence of steps. For example, they cannot pretend to fold a letter, place it into an envelope, address the envelope, and then affix a stamp. In contrast to ideomotor apraxia, which is associated with nonfluent aphasia, ideational apraxia is almost inseparable from dementia. In particular, ideational apraxia is a hallmark of frontotemporal dementia, where it reflects executive dysfunction. Alzheimer disease and multiple strokes, because they lead to dementia and impaired planning and execution, typically cause ideational apraxia.

In other sections, this book covers several other apraxias: Construction and dressing apraxias, which are typically manifestations of nondominant-hemisphere lesions (see later), and gait apraxia, which is a hallmark of normal-pressure hydrocephalus (see Fig. 7-10).

Nondominant-Hemisphere Syndromes

Neuropsychologic symptoms arising from nondominant-hemisphere injury – nondominant symptoms – tend to be short-lived and subtle. They occur not only individually, but also in various combinations and in interrelated patterns. Perhaps more than with other manifestations of cerebral injury, patients’ premorbid intelligence, personality, and defense mechanisms determine their expression. Detecting them requires considerable clinical acumen and gentle probing. Physicians confronting patients with neurologic deficits that they do not accept risk precipitating a catastrophic reaction.

Their cause is almost always a structural lesion, such as trauma, stroke, or malignant tumor that has rapidly developed in the nondominant parietal or frontal lobe cortex, underlying thalamus, and reticular activating system. Damage to these regions impairs arousal and distribution of spatial attention.

Nondominant-hemisphere syndromes appear abruptly and unexpectedly. They often leave patients perplexed because they cannot appreciate their deficits. Patients often turn to classic defense mechanisms to protect their sense of self or ego. Most often, they deny that they have a hemiparesis, blindness, or other neurologic deficit. Their denial may be implicit rather than explicit. For example, blind people may walk around hospital corridors as though they remain sighted. Projecting a conflict between their own self-image and the reality of having a stroke-induced hemiparesis to another individual, for example, patients may say that their deficit is really their roommate’s who sustained an obvious stroke. They may also rationalize their problem. For example, when asked why he did not move his left arm, one patient stated, “I don’t want to. If I wanted to move it, I would.” Another stroke victim explained that she did not move her arm because of pain, not paresis, and that if her doctors gave her adequate pain relief, she would obviously be able to move it. Sometimes laughing off the deficit, patients humorously avoid dealing with their loss of body function and its implications.

For whatever reason patients cannot accept a neurologic deficit, their misperceptions prevent them from complying with hospital routines. Patients with nondominant-hemisphere syndromes frequently refuse to participate in rehabilitation programs because they feel no need for them and participating would force them to confront their deficits. Not acknowledging their deficits also leads to potentially dangerous behavior, such as blind patients planning to drive home. They typically refuse to make realistic discharge plans. Even if encounters with medical staff and family do not precipitate a catastrophic reaction, patients with nondominant disorders tend to be bellicose when conversations approach their illness, deficit, and need for realistic plans.

Hemi-Inattention

Patients with nondominant-hemisphere injury most prominently display hemi-inattention (hemispatial neglect). They typically ignore visual, tactile, and other sensory stimuli that originate from their left side (Fig. 8-6). For example, they disregard, fail to perceive, or misinterpret objects in their left visual field (Fig. 8-7). Sometimes men with this condition leave the left side of their face unshaven. In contrast to patients with homonymous hemianopsia, who usually develop some awareness and thus make compensatory movements to keep objects in the preserved visual field, those with hemi-inattention remain oblivious to their situation and make no normal exploratory eye or limb movements.

Another manifestation of hemi-inattention – extinction on double simultaneous stimulation [DSS] – occurs when an examiner stimulates both sides of a patient’s body, but the patient pays no attention to the left-sided stimulation. For example, when the examiner touches the left arm, the patient correctly reports that it was touched, but when the examiner touches both arms, the patient reports that only the right one was touched. Another example of DSS can occur with visual stimulation. A patient might correctly perceive a flash of light in the left visual field, but when the examiner simultaneously flashes light in both visual fields, the patient would report seeing it only in the right-sided field.

Anosognosia

Anosognosia – a term constructed by Babinski from the Greek (a, without; nosos, disease; gnosis, knowledge) – has come to describe an inability to acknowledge or a denial of a physical deficit. The classic example is denial of a left hemiparesis. Often an integral part of hemi-inattention, anosognosia carries great clinical importance.

Patients with anosognosia typically cannot identify the affected part of their own body (somatotopagnosia or autotopagnosia). For example, they might claim (deny) that the examiner’s limb is really theirs; do not recognize their own paretic limb; or refuse to accept that the obviously paralyzed limb is even weak. Sometimes they attribute (project) the weakened limb to a third person, such as another patient. That ploy holds instant appeal if that person has an immobilized limb from a stroke or fracture. Alternatively, even while accepting their hand’s weakness, patients might offer an improbable explanation (rationalize), such as that they merely fell asleep on it and its strength will return in a few hours, or that they simply do not wish to move it.

A physician finding anosognosia in a stroke holds more than academic interest. It complicates and delays recovery. Physicians might slowly and on several occasions introduce the idea that the patient has limitations in certain functions, but with participation in rehabilitation and the passage of time, improvement will occur.

Neurologists attribute anosognosia to a loss of afferent sensory input, particularly loss of proprioception. Clinical experience, however, shows that, while loss of sensation may be a prerequisite, it alone is insufficient. For example, patients with a thoracic spinal cord transection lose feeling in their lower trunk and legs, but they remain acutely aware of their paraparesis and sensory loss. Another theory attributes nondominant symptoms to disordered attentiveness and arousal arising from damage of the underlying thalamus and reticular activating system. In any case, patients’ premorbid personality and emotional state do not predict that they might develop this condition.

Aprosody

Aprosody constitutes the inability to recognize emotional or affective qualities of other individual’s speech. Thus, nondominant-hemisphere lesions interfere with patients’ capacity to feel emotions from others’ tone of voice. For example, a patient with aprosody would be unable to appreciate the contrasting feelings in the question “Are you going home?” asked first by a jealous hospital roommate and then by a gleeful child. On a more subtle level, patients also might not perceive the emotions conveyed by a spouse.

In addition, aprosody restricts the ability to impart emotional qualities to speech. With no inflection or style, patients’ speech sounds bland and unfeeling.

To assess prosody, the examiner refashions a short version of the aphasia examination. During spontaneous speech, the examiner notes the patient’s variations in volume, pitch, and emphasis. The examiner might have the patient ask a question, such as “May I have the ball?” in the manner of a friend and then a stern schoolteacher using appropriate vocal and facial expressions. The examiner then asks a similar question, impersonating the various characters while the patient tries to identify them. As an alternative, the examiner might ask the patient to describe pictures of people obviously displaying emotions.

Loss of nonverbal communication tends to accompany aprosody. In particular, patients lose meaningful face and limb expressions, popularly called “body language” or technically called speech’s paralinguistic component. These physical aspects of communication lend conviction, emphasis, and affect to spoken words. Indeed, such physical expressions seem independent and sometimes more credible than speech. Well-known examples are children crossing their fingers when promising, adults who wink while telling a joke, and people who smile while relating sad events.

Extending the idea that the nondominant hemisphere confers affect on language, several authors have suggested that the nondominant hemisphere governs perception and expression of emotion and other complex nonverbal processes. The dominant hemisphere, they suggest, is responsible for verbal, sequential, and analytic cognitive processes.

Disconnection Syndromes

Almost all mental processes require communication pathways between two or more cerebral cortical areas located within one cerebral hemisphere, and many require communication between areas in opposite hemispheres. The arcuate fasciculus, for example, provides intrahemispheric communication between Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas. Thick myelin-coated axonal (white-matter) bundles, often called commissures, provide interhemispheric communication. Of them, the corpus callosum is the most conspicuous and important.

Injuries that sever communication pathways, but spare the actual area, cause uncommon but interesting phenomena, the disconnection syndromes. Neurologists predicted their existence before verifying them in actual patients, much as physicists have predicted certain subatomic particles before demonstrating them. This chapter has previously discussed several disconnection syndromes: (1) alexia without agraphia; (2) conduction aphasia; and (3) ideomotor apraxia with its varieties, buccofacial and limb apraxia. Subsequent chapters will present other disconnection syndromes, including the medial longitudinal fasciculus syndrome, also known as intranuclear ophthalmoplegia (see Chapters 12 and 15).

The anterior cerebral artery syndrome results from an occlusion of both anterior cerebral arteries that causes an infarction of both frontal lobes and the anterior corpus callosum. In this syndrome, information cannot pass between the left hemisphere language centers and the right hemisphere motor centers. Although the patient’s left arm and leg will have normal spontaneous movement, those limbs fail to follow an examiner’s verbal or written requests to move them. In other words, the patient will have unilateral (left-sided) limb apraxia (see Fig. 8-5).

Other injuries of the corpus callosum surprisingly may not produce disconnection syndromes. For example, the corpus callosum occasionally fails to develop in utero (congenital absence), but unless other anomalies are present, patients may have no overt impairment. In Marchiafava–Bignami syndrome or disease, which has been attributed, in a minority of cases, to excessive consumption of Italian red wine, patients may show disconnection signs, but only as part of extensive cerebral dysfunction.

Split-Brain Syndrome

The most important disconnection syndrome referable to the corpus callosum is the splitbrain syndrome. Now rare, this disorder previously most often resulted from a longitudinal surgical division of the corpus callosum (commissurotomy) performed by neurosurgeons in an effort to control intractable epilepsy (see Chapter 10). The commissurotomy almost completely isolated each cerebral hemisphere.

In cases of the split-brain syndrome, examiners may present certain information to only a single, isolated hemisphere. For example, examiners can show pictures, writing, and other visual information in one of the patient’s visual fields to present information to only the contralateral hemisphere (Fig. 8-9). Likewise, by having a blindfolded patient touch objects with the one hand, examiners can present tactile information to only the contralateral hemisphere. However, auditory information cannot be presented exclusively – only predominantly – to one hemisphere. (Because pathways are duplicated in the brainstem [see Fig. 4-16], sounds presented to one ear travel, after the medial geniculate synapse, to both hemispheres, but predominantly to the contralateral one.)

The interruption of the corpus callosum prevents the right hemisphere from sharing most information with the entire brain, particularly the left hemisphere’s language centers. Thus, the right hemisphere’s information, experience, and emotion cannot reach the patients’ consciousness, at least at the level of verbal expression. For example, if an object is placed in a blindfolded patient’s left hand, the patient cannot name or describe it, and the right hand cannot choose an identical object. Similarly, if one hand learns to follow a maze, the other hand will have to be taught separately.

Not only can each hemisphere separately perceive visual and tactile information, but also each can separately perceive emotions. For example, if a humorous picture were shown to the right visual field, a patient might laugh and be able to describe the picture’s humorous content; however, if the same picture were shown to the patient’s left visual field, it might provoke an amused response but one that the patient could not verbalize or even fully comprehend. If a sad picture were shown in the left visual field and a humorous one in the right, the patient’s amused response would be distorted because of the conflict.

Split-brain studies have suggested that normal people have, in their two hemispheres, neuropsychologic systems that are independent, parallel, and capable of simultaneous reasoning. They also show that, in normal people, the systems usually complement each other, but they potentially conflict.

References

Benton AL. Gerstmann’s syndrome. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:445–447.

Besson M, Schon D. Comparison between language and music. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2001;930:232–358.

Brust JC. Music and the neurologist. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2001;930:143–152.

Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, et al. Hemispatial neglect. Neurology. 2004;62:749–756.

Croquelois A, Bogousslavsky J. Stroke aphasia. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2011;31:392–399.

Faber R, Abrams R, Taylor MA, et al. Comparison of schizophrenic patients with formal thought disorder and neurologically impaired patients with aphasia. Am J Psychiatry. 1983;140:1348–1351.

Feinberg TE, Schindler RJ, Flanagan NG, et al. Two alien hand syndromes. Neurology. 1992;42:19–24.

Greenberg VD. Freud and his Aphasia Book: Language and the Sources of Psychoanalysis. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1998.

Greener J, Enderby P, Whurr R. Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2, 2000. CD000425

Hemenway D. Bimanual dexterity in baseball players. N Engl J Med. 1983;309:1587.

Hickok G, Bellugi U, Klima ES. Sign language in the brain. Sci Am. 2001;June:59–65.

Hills AE. Aphasia: Progress in the last quarter of a century. Neurology. 2007;69:200–213.

Kikkert MA, Ribbers GM, Koudstaal PJ. Alien hand syndrome in stroke: A report of 2 cases and review of the literature. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006;87:728–732.

Laurent-Vannier A, Pradat-Diehl P, Chevignard M, et al. Spatial and motor neglect in children. Neurology. 2003;60:202–207.

Motley MT. Slips of the tongue. Sci Am. 1985;253:116–125.

Orfei MD, Robinson RG, Prigatano GP, et al. Anosognosia for hemiplegia after stroke is a multifaceted phenomenon: A systemic review of the literature. Brain. 2007;130:3075–3090.

Portal JM, Romano PE. Patterns of eye–hand dominance in baseball players. N Engl J Med. 1988;319:655.

Shinnar S, Rapin I, Arnold S, et al. Language regression in childhood. Pediatr Neurol. 2001;24:183–189.

Vocat R, Staub F, Stroppni T, et al. Anosognosia for hemiplegia: A clinical-anatomic prospective study. Brain. 2010;133:3578–3597.

Chapter 8 Questions and Answers

1–5. Formulate the following cases:

Answers:

The examiner was uncertain whether the patient had depression, dementia, or other neuropsychologic abnormality. Please discuss the case and suggest further evaluation.

Answer:

The patient was probably exposed to excessive carbon monoxide and had resultant cerebral anoxia. As in many cases of survival following cardiac arrest or strangulation, cerebral anoxia creates irreparable cerebral cortex damage. When patients permanently lose all consciousness, cognitive ability, and voluntary motor function, neurologists judge them to be in the persistent vegetative state (see Chapter 11).

In this case, the cerebral damage was incomplete. Anoxia probably damaged the entire cerebral cortex except for the perisylvian language arc, which is comprised of Wernicke’s area, the arcuate fasciculus, and Broca’s area. Isolation of this crucial region from the rest of the cerebral cortex causes transcortical or isolation aphasia, which permits repetition of words and phrases, no matter how complex. In this disorder, language formation does not interact with the rest of the brain’s language system, and patients cannot name objects or follow requests. Moreover, because the anoxia damages a large portion of the cerebral cortex, patients usually have dementia, paresis, frontal release signs, and cortical blindness.

When cerebral cortex damage is superimposed on depressive illness or other psychiatric disease, the clinical picture is unpredictable. Psychiatrists should begin with language function testing in psychiatric patients with such insults.

In isolation aphasia, which is usually caused by cerebral hypoperfusion, the most vulnerable portions of the cerebral cortex are damaged. In contrast, because the perisylvian language arc is well perfused, it escapes damage. Language processes may continue within this preserved region, but they receive no input from other regions of the cerebral cortex.

Answer:

b. The patient has both mild but definite anomic aphasia, which is a fluent aphasia, and dementia that is probably inherited. Unlike a structural lesion’s causing most cases of aphasia, the cause in this case, as in many cases of anomic aphasia, is probably the frontotemporal dementia. This neurodegenerative illness develops earlier and progresses more rapidly than Alzheimer disease. Moreover, even though Alzheimer disease follows a genetic pattern in many families, frontotemporal dementia more frequently follows one. In it, behavioral changes or language deficits accompany or overshadow cognitive impairment.